Dear reader of ADxS.org, please excuse the disruption.

ADxS.org needs about $63500 in 2024. In 2023 we received donations of about $ 32200. Unfortunately, 99.8% of our readers do not donate. If everyone who reads this request makes a small contribution, our fundraising campaign for 2024 would be over after a few days. This donation request is displayed 23,000 times a week, but only 75 people donate. If you find ADxS.org useful, please take a minute and support ADxS.org with your donation. Thank you!

Since 01.06.2021 ADxS.org is supported by the non-profit ADxS e.V..

$8975 of $63500 - as of 2024-02-29
14%
Header Image
The subtypes of ADHD: ADHD-HI, ADHD-C (mixed type), ADHD-I (ADD), ADHD-RI (restricted inattentive) and others

Sitemap

The subtypes of ADHD: ADHD-HI, ADHD-C (mixed type), ADHD-I (ADD), ADHD-RI (restricted inattentive) and others

Since DSM 5, the subtypes have been called presentation forms.
DSM 5 distinguishes between three forms of presentation:

  • ADHD-HI (predominant hyperactivity)
  • ADHD-I (predominant inattention) and
  • ADHD-C (attention problems and hyperactivity in equal measure)

A further form of presentation is being discussed:

  • ADHD-RI (restrictive inattention)

ADHD-HI is characterized by predominant hyperactivity and impulsivity. Those affected often have difficulty controlling their impulses and frequently react excessively. Social problems are common because ADHD-HI sufferers often do not take other people’s feelings into account. Comorbid disorders are often externalizing and there are more frequent inflammatory problems and a flattened cortisol stress response. ADHD-HI responds well to methylphenidate (MPH).

ADHD-I (ADD) is characterized by predominant inattention and occurs mainly in childhood and adolescence. ADHD-I often shows daydreaming, difficulty concentrating, working memory problems and aversion to noisy group situations. Comorbid disorders are usually internalizing, such as anxiety disorders or depression.

ADHD-C (mixed type) is a mixture of ADHD-I and ADHD-HI. Those affected show strong symptoms in both core areas of ADHD, namely inattention on the one hand AND hyperactivity / impulsivity on the other.

The pure ADHD-HI type is usually diagnosed up to the age of 6, 7 years at the latest, exceptionally up to 14, 15 years and only rarely occurs later. This is probably mainly due to the fact that inattention symptoms can only be reliably diagnosed from this age onwards. ADHD-C could therefore be referred to as the ADHD-HI type in later development. In adults with ADHD, attention problems can also significantly diminish or disappear, even if they diminish considerably less often than hyperactivity problems: ADHD in adults.
The ICD 10 distinguishes between a simple activity and attention disorder (F90.0) and a hyperkinetic disorder of social behavior (F90.1), for which a social behavior disorder is also required. According to DSM, the latter is (in our opinion rightly) considered a comorbidity. ICD 11 has come closer to the DSM.
The presentation forms (subtypes) ADHD-HI, ADHD-I and ADHD-C have been shown to be valid, while the evidence for a subtype of Hyperkinetic Disorder of Social Behavior is insufficient1

SCT is no longer considered a subtype of ADHD. As SCT appears to be closely related and highly comorbid with ADHD, we have dedicated a separate article to SCT: SCT - Sluggish Cognitive Tempo.

In adults, a gender-independent frequency distribution of the subtypes was found, from:

  • ADHD-C: 56 %2 to 62 %3
  • ADHD-I: 31 %3 to 37 %2
  • ADHD-HI: 2 %2 to 7 %3

The (genetic) causes and neurophysiological processes are very similar in all forms of presenile dementia (subtypes). To date, only a few truly reliable distinguishing criteria are known. The most relevant difference appears to be the endocrine response to acute stress. As the stress-induced neurotransmitter releases (particularly noradrenaline) appear to run parallel to these subtype-specific cortisol responses to acute stress, this could explain some of the differences between ADHD-HI/ADHD-C and ADHD-I.

We regard the ADHD subtypes as different (psychological) forms of reaction to one and the same genetic / neurological source of disorder, whereby essentially the personality traits

  • Extroverted / introverted
  • Neuroticism
    and next to it
  • The personal way of processing stress (BIS/BAS) as a phenotypic stress reaction and
  • Learning to deal with stress (role model)

determine which subtype an ADHD sufferer develops. These are reflected neurophysiologically in the cortisol stress response. More on this below under 1.2.3.1.

The subtype expression of those affected is not necessarily stable throughout their lives.4 We know a number of sufferers who report a significant change in subtype.

1. Presentation forms (subtypes) of ADHD according to symptoms

The specialist literature primarily mentions 3 presentation forms (subtypes) of ADHD: the ADHD-I type (1.1.), the ADHD-HI type (see 1.2.) and ADHD-C (see 1.3). While the ADHD-HI subtype is probably only an early form of the mixed type, sluggish cognitive tempo (SCT) is becoming increasingly clear as an independent disorder. The sporadically further described forms are unlikely to be true subtypes and are only mentioned for the sake of completeness.

Regardless of the typing of ADHD-I, ADHD-HI and ADHD-C based on the observed symptoms, there is a typing that is based on the different EEG patterns measured ADHD subtypes according to EEG. This has not yet become established, although it could be measurable using objective biomarkers. So far, there is a lack of experience about the different symptoms of the EEG subtypes and how they react to specific treatments. An attempt to automatically differentiate the presentation forms based on their EEG patterns using AI failed.5 Moreover, only 84% of ADHD sufferers could be distinguished from those not affected.

1.1. ADHD-I: predominantly inattentive type

1.1.1. Other names of the ADHD-I subtype

  • ADHD-I (the term we use today)
  • ADS (colloquial, used by us in the past)
  • ADD (older international colloquial term)
  • ADHD-PI (predominantly inattentive)
  • Hypoactive (Prof. Simchen)
  • Dreamer (colloquially descriptive)
  • Underactivated (distractible due to boredom)

1.1.2. Specific symptoms of ADHD-I

ADHD is a symptom cluster. The symptoms do not clearly differentiate between the presentation forms (subtypes). In ADHD-I, inattention predominates over hyperactivity, impulsivity and inner restlessness.

  • 3 to 6 years6

    • Requests are not heeded
      • Therefore does not follow requests
    • Dreamy
    • Stares into space
    • Finds it difficult to occupy himself with something
      • Daydreaming
      • Thoughts wander
    • Loud group situations tend to be perceived as unpleasant
    • Reluctance to change familiar processes
    • Aversion to quick transitions, needs to adjust to new things first
  • 6 to 10 years:7

    • Inattention
      • Can therefore not follow some lessons
    • Cannot understand some of the subject matter
    • Concentration difficulties
      • On teacher presentation
      • On own work
    • Looks dreamy
    • Lack of active cooperation
  • 10 to 20 years:7

    • Possible improvement in attention due to subsequent development of the brain
    • Attention problems remain at least partially present
      and are still perceived as very stressful
  • Regardless of age:

    • The ADHD-I subtype is more introverted (in contrast to the more extroverted ADHD-HI subtype)8
    • Internalizing9
    • Shy10
    • Increased susceptibility to internalizing disorders11
      e.g.
      • Anxiety disorders
      • Depression
    • Social problems of the ADHD-I subtype arise from8
      • Too shy
      • Passivity
      • Introversion
      • Being still
    • Disorders primarily in working memory and auditory processing, which places high demands on working memory.12
    • More frequent comorbid disorders than ADHD-HI subtype13
    • Partial performance disorders more frequent
      • Reading and spelling difficulties
      • Dyscalculia (dyscalculia)
    • Often perform better in spatial thinking and artistic drawing than in verbal skills.14
    • More bored than distracted.15
    • Motivational problems rather than problems with inhibition.16
    • Tends to have a high level of self-awareness.16
    • Lethargic states (to be distinguished from depression)17
    • A significant subsection of ADHD-I is said to have slowed thinking (Sluggish)16
      We consider the term “slowed-down thinking” to be inaccurate and inappropriate. We see slowed decision-making. The ability to think quickly is basically present; we suspect an excessive blockade of the PFC by noradrenaline and possibly other neurotransmitters via the alpha-1 adrenoceptor. SCT is no longer considered a subtype of ADHD, nor is it considered specific to ADHD-I.
    • More frequent allergies due to the excessive cortisol reaction. Cortisol promotes the immune defense against external stresses.
    • Corresponding disorders of hypercortisolism are18
      • Depression
      • Anxiety disorder
      • Anorexia
      • Alcoholism
      • Metabolic syndrome
    • Learning disorders
      • More frequently than with ADHD-HI9
      • A high cortisol response to acute stress, which is typical of ADHD-I, correlates with poorer memory performance when learning vocabulary after exposure to stress - but only in men. Women could be protected against this by their sex hormones.19 Cortisol administration also worsened learning performance.
        Cortisol, which is often elevated as a stress response in ADHD-I, blocks the retrieval of declarative (explicit) memory via the glucocorticoid receptors (GR) in the PFC and hippocampus. The non-declarative (implicit, intuitive) memory is not impaired.20 This could explain the more frequent thinking and memory blocks in ADHD-I and also why ADHD-I sufferers often seem to have a higher level of intuition. In any case, it would stand to reason that the shift in the focus of memory skills leads to a shift in problem-solving patterns. Trappmann-Korr calls this “holistic” perception.
        It is likely that not only the retrieval (remembering), but also the acquisition (learning) and memory consolidation (long-term storage) of information is impaired. Consolidation occurs particularly during sleep in the first half of the night, which is characterized by particularly low basal cortisol levels. Consolidation can be prevented by low cortisol levels.20
    • Attention problems
      • Sustained attention (ADHD-HI and ADHD-I subtype)
      • Selective attention (ADHD-I subtype only)21
  • Negative symptom differentiation in ADHD-I:

    • Externalizing behaviour disorders and aggressive behaviour are atypical in ADHD-I and indicate the ADHD-HI subtype.22.

      • Less frequent symptoms of aggressive or oppositional defiant behavior2324
    • ADHD-I is diagnosed less frequently because it is less likely to be noticed unpleasantly due to the “comfortable” symptoms of internalized stress management for the environment. In addition, research suffers from the fact that many studies do not differentiate the results according to forms of presentation (subtypes) and in particular do not differentiate ADHD-C from the ADHD-I type.

    • Less frequent comorbid oppositional defiant behavior than in ADHD-HI subtype.8

    • Lower proportion of smokers than in ADHD-HI subtype (the pathways of action of nicotine and methylphenidate are similar)16

    • Less frequent inflammatory reactions such as atopic dermatitis due to the excessive cortisol response. Cortisol inhibits the inflammatory processes initiated by CRH and instead promotes defense against foreign bodies, which can manifest itself as allergies if excessive

More on the required frequency and intensity of ADHD symptoms at Symptoms of ADHD in children by age.

1.1.3. Peculiarities of the medication

  • MPH responding
    • According to one view, a noticeable proportion of methylphenidate non-responders (MPH does not work). If MPH is effective, low doses are often sufficient.16
    • According to another opinion, ADHD-HI and ADHD-I do not differ in the MPH response rate.25
  • A significant proportion of ADHD-I sufferers respond to amphetamine medication rather than MPH.
  • It should also be helpful:
    • Nortriptyline
    • Bupropion (if stronger activation is required)

1.1.4. Neurophysiological characteristics of ADHD-I

1.1.4.1. Increased cortisol stress response in ADHD-I

ADHD-I often appears to be associated with a particularly strong cortisol response to acute stress. Changes in cortisol levels in ADHD in: Cortisol and other stress hormones in ADHD

1.1.4.2. Further biomarkers for ADHD-I
1.1.4.2.1. Stress responses of cortisol, noradrenaline and dopamine

Underactivation of the PFC in ADHD-I is explained by the fact that the stress responses of cortisol, noradrenaline and dopamine correlate in the brain. More on this at Neurotransmitters during stress
* A strong increase in noradrenaline / dopamine shuts down the PFC. This deactivation of the PFC occurs via alpha-1 adrenoceptors, which have a lower noradrenaline and cortisol affinity than alpha-2 adrenoceptors and are therefore only addressed at very high noradrenaline and cortisol levels.2627282930
A particularly strong increase in DA and NE during severe stress could therefore lead to a (frequent) underactivation of the PFC, as is typical in ADHD-I.
This could explain Raynaud’s and high blood pressure problems in some ADHD-I sufferers, which are also mediated by alpha-1 adrenoceptors.
The question is whether alpha-1-adrenoceptor antagonists, which are successfully used against Raynaud’s and high blood pressure, might not also be helpful against PFC blockades in ADHD-I.
To date, only alpha-2 adrenoceptor agonists have been used, which are likely to be considered third-line drugs. Guanfacine addresses alpha-2-A and alpha-2-D, yohimbine alpha-2-B adrenoceptors. The agonization of the more affine alpha-2 receptors is contrary in effect to the antagonization of the alpha-1 receptors. As with the cortisol receptors, the less affine receptor is responsible for switching off the system and is only addressed at very high messenger substance levels. If the more affine receptors are too pronounced, the less affine switch-off receptors are not activated.
Guanfacine and yohimbine occupy the more affine alpha-2 receptors, leaving more neurotransmitter for the less affine alpha-1 receptors, which are therefore more easily addressed.

1.1.4.2.2. CRH

CRH also has an effect on the PFC and can impair it at high CRH levels.
Increased cortisol responses to a stressor correlate with an increased variance in response time.31 An increased variance in response times could be explained by an impairment in the performance of the PFC, which is particularly pronounced in the ADHD-I subtype. This impairment could be explained by increased noradrenaline responses to acute stress, analogous to the cortisol response. Some diagnosticians pay particular attention to this variance in response times when diagnosing ADHD-I.
Cortisol leads to a decrease in cortisol and noradrenaline
This negative feedback is the result of cortisol (it shuts down the HPA axis again) and is independent of the synchronicity of the release of cortisol and noradrenaline in response to stress

1.1.4.2.3. Dopamine receptors

The dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) has a special significance in the PFC, which is why Diamond32 assumes that a disruption of the DRD4 7R gene is associated with ADHD-I. We do not believe this to be the case.
Noble found that the gene polymorphisms A1, B2 and intron 6 1 of DRD2 and the DRD4 7R polymorphism were associated with an increased Novelty Seeking (sensation seeking) score. However, none of these polymorphisms correlated with a high Harm Avoidance Score (BIS), as is typical for ADHD-I.33
High Novelty Seeking / Sensation Seeking rates are associated with high aversion to boredom and correlate with impulse control disorders.34 Boredom is a cause of inattention in ADHD-I, while impulse control problems are atypical of ADHD-I and more typical of ADHD-HI.

1.1.4.2.4. Serotonin

In ADHD-I, there is often said to be a lack of serotonin.35
One study found strong association of the L/L genotype of the 5-HTTLPR gene with ADHD-C and ADHD-HI, while the 5-HTTLPR-L/L genotype showed no difference.36
The 5-HTTLPR-L/L genotype correlates with hyperactive symptoms.37
One study reports a correlation between the presence of a 5-HTTLPR-S allele and inattention.38

1.1.4.2.5. Orexin A

ADHD-I may have lower levels of Orexin A than the ADHD-HI subtype and than non-affected individuals.39 More about orexin at Orexin / hypocretin In the section Hormones in ADHD in the chapter Neurological aspects.

1.1.5. EEG image for ADHD-I

See below under “Subtypes of ADHD according to EEG”.

1.1.6. Sluggish Cognitive Tempo (SCT) - independent disorder

SCT is now regarded as a separate disorder independent of ADHD, even though SCT and ADHD are very often comorbid. However, there are SCT sufferers without ADHD.
More about SCT at SCT - Sluggish Cognitive Tempo.

1.2. ADHD-HI: predominantly hyperactive/impulsive subtype

1.2.1. Other names

  • ADHD-HI (used by us)
  • ADHD (colloquially an imprecise collective term for all subtypes)
  • ADHD (English; used internationally, but also as a collective term for all subtypes)
  • ADHD-PH (predominantly hyperactive/impulsive)
  • ADHD-H/I (hyperactive/impulsive)
  • Fidget spinner (colloquially descriptive)

1.2.2. Specific symptoms

  • Clear expression of
    • Hyperactivity
    • Impulsiveness
  • No inattention
    • Inattention is added to hyperactivity / impulsivity: ADHD-C, see below
  • Social problems very common
    arise because ADHD-HI subtype
    • Offends others
    • Takes things away from them
    • Can’t wait for his turn
    • Is too direct and authoritative
    • Behaves disruptively and
    • Acts without considering the feelings of others40
  • More extroverted (in contrast to the more introverted ADHD-I subtype)98
  • Frequent behavioral disorders and (comorbid) aggressive behavior41.
  • More frequent comorbidity with oppositional defiant behavior16
  • Disorders primarily in the area of behavioral inhibition16 and executive functions42
  • Is more distractible than bored16
  • Has an inhibition problem rather than a motivation problem.16
  • Tendency towards low self-awareness16
  • Good response to methylphenidate.
    The non-responder rate (MPH does not work) should be 10 %.43
  • The striatum is primarily affected. The fact that DAT1 plays an important role in the striatum and MPH primarily acts on the DAT explains the good effect of MPH in ADHD-HI.16
  • Higher proportion of smokers than in the ADHD-I subtype. (Nicotine acts as a stimulant like methylphenidate)16
  • Hyperactive/impulsive type often only a precursor in childhood/adolescence for later ADHD-C4445
  • Boys are affected 5 times more often than girls46.
  • More frequent inflammatory reactions than in the ADHD-I subtype due to the flattened cortisol response to stress. Cortisol inhibits the inflammatory response mediated by CRH.
  • The corresponding symptoms of hypocortisolism are18
    • Atypical depression
    • Inflammation problems47
      • E.g. irritable bowel syndrome
      • E.g. neurodermatitis
    • Tiredness
      Cortisol inhibits the pro-inflammatory effect of CRH
      promotes cytokines such as interleukin 1 and 6; thus (as with fever) “sickness behavior”:
      • Fatigue
      • Lack of initiative
      • Tiredness
      • E.g. Chronic Fatigue Syndrome
      • E.g. burnout
    • Sensitivity to pain
      Cortisol inhibits prostaglandin synthesis (= disinhibition of prostaglandins)
      Prostaglandins modulate pain perception
      this systemically lowers the pain threshold (pain symptoms “migrate”)
      Hypocortisolism can promote inflammatory processes in the spinal cord (chronic pain)
      • E.g. fibromyalgia48
      • E.g. chronic lower abdominal pain
    • Stress intolerance
      Cortisol inhibits locus coeruleus and thus reduces the release of noradrenaline in the CNS
      A low cortisol response results in limited noradrenaline inhibition = loss of an important stress brake
      Hypocortisolism thus causes stress intolerance, irritability, sensitivity to sensory stimuli (noise, etc.)
      Hypocortisolism could promote the development of intrusions as observed in PTSD49

Negative symptom differentiation in ADHD-HI:

  • If there is inattention and hyperactivity (the latter also as an inner drive), this is known as ADHD-C.
    Inattention can usually only be detected between the ages of 6 and 15. ADHD-HI (with hyperactivity without attention problems) can therefore be regarded as an early form of the later mixed type.4450
    The question of whether ADHD exists entirely without inattention has not been conclusively answered. We tend to assume that there is, although this is likely to be a rather rare manifestation.
    The higher the aptitude, the better the coping mechanisms. The older the affected person is, the more remitted individual symptoms can be, which is why a lack of (well-hidden) inattention in test settings is quite common. Similarly, a high level of intrinsic interest in the tests on the part of those affected leads to an equalization of test performance compared to those not affected. This is plausible if the stress benefit and thus the mechanisms of the stress symptom of inattention are considered.
  • Allergies appear to occur less frequently in ADHD-HI than in the ADHD-I subtype due to the more frequently flattened cortisol response to stress. Cortisol increases the immune response to external stresses such as allergens.

1.2.3. Neurophysiological characteristics of ADHD-HI / ADHD-C

In ADHD-HI (with hyperactivity/impulsivity) and also in correlating aggression, the release of catecholamines and cortisol in response to acute stressors is often reduced or absent compared to non-affected individuals, whereas the cortisol stress response is typically increased in ADHD-I affected individuals compared to non-affected individuals.

1.2.3.1. Flattened cortisol stress response in ADHD-HI / ADHD-C

ADHD-HI is often associated with a flattened cortisol response to acute stress. Cortisol in ADHD

1.2.3.2. Deficient HPA axis deactivation in ADHD-HI / ADHD-C

Since cortisol not only mediates stress symptoms but also switches off the HPA axis, a reduced cortisol stress response leads to a lack of braking of the HPA stress system.
The HPA axis / stress regulation axis And The autonomic nervous system.

1.2.3.3. Reduced noradrenaline reduction in ADHD-HI / ADHD-C?

Cortisol also causes a reduction in noradrenaline.
A too low cortisol response to acute stress and a resulting too low norepinephrine degradation could possibly explain a permanent overactivation of the PFC in ADHD-HI/ADHD-C.
A slightly increased (dopamine and) noradrenaline level in the PFC increases its activation and results in improved cognitive performance.5152535455 Only a strong increase in noradrenaline / dopamine reduces the PFC.2627282930

1.2.3.4. Reduced hippocampus volume in ADHD-HI / ADHD-C

A fairly extensive study reported reduced hippocampal volume in ADHD-HI as opposed to ADHD-I and non-affected individuals.
* ADHD-HI: Reduction in the hippocampus areas56
* CA1
* CA4
* Molecular layer
* Granule cell band of the dentate gyrus
* Presubiculum
* Subiculum
* Hippocampal tail
* Other hippocampal regions were not reduced in size
* ADHD-I: no reliable differences in hippocampal volume compared to controls56
* Finally, smaller hippocampal areas correlated with higher behavioral ADHD indices. For example, a smaller subiculum correlated with a higher overall ADHD-HI index and higher hyperactivity/impulsivity and lower IQ.56

1.2.3.5. Indole acetic acid for ADHD-HI / ADHD-C

In ADHD-HI sufferers with comorbid depressive symptoms, one study found significantly higher morning than evening levels of indoleacetic acid. MPH reduced this by 50 %. MPH also reduced the morning levels of indolepropionic acid and brought the daily profile back to the levels of healthy control subjects.57

  • SHR show behavioral subgroups corresponding to ADHD-HI and ADHD-I
    • One study found subgroups of SHR (Spontaneous(ly) hypertensive rat) that differed significantly in terms of impulsivity. Impulsive SHRs showed a significant difference compared to non-impulsive SHRs and WKYs (as controls; no behavioral subgroups were found)58
      • Reduced noradrenaline levels
        • In the cingulate cortex
        • In the medial-frontal cortex
      • Reduced serotonin turnover
        • In the medial-frontal cortex
      • Reduced density of CB1 cannabinoid receptors
        • In the PFC
        • Acute administration of a cannabinoid agonist reduced impulsivity in impulsive SHR, with no change in WKY
1.2.3.6. TSH, serum ferritin, lactic acid in hyperactivity (ADHD-HI / ADHD-C)

In 49 children with ADHD with and without hyperactivity:59

  • TSH correlated with Conners 3 scale values (ADHD)
    • so clearly that diagnostic use could be conceivable
  • TSH, serum ferritin and lactic acid correlated with HI value
  • TSH and lactic acid were independently associated with HI

1.2.4. EEG image in ADHD-HI

See below under Presentation forms (subtypes) in ADHD according to EEG.

1.3. ADHD-C - mixed type of ADHD-HI and ADHD-I

  • Clear manifestation in both core symptom areas:
    • Hyperactivity (in children; in adults; inner restlessness) AND impulsivity
    • Inattention
      • Problems with sustained attention (ADHD-C and ADHD-I)
      • Fewer problems with selective attention (ADHD-I subtype only)21
  • Largely corresponds to hyperkinetic disorder according to ICD-10.2.3.1 Subgroups of ADHD-C

A larger study of adolescents found three subgroups of ADHD-C in which the specific symptoms were also reflected in fMRI-measurable changes in brain structure in the brain systems known for these cognitive functions.60

A small study replicated previous research that frontal delta and theta power is higher in ADHD-C than ADHD-I and TD groups.61

1.3.1. ADHD-C with inhibition deficits, without reward deficits

Inhibition deficits refers to deficits in executive functions and inhibitory cognitive functions.

Underfunction within

  • Different frontal lobes
  • Parietal lobe
  • Subcortical
  • Cerebellar regions

in the inhibition of motor reactions

and

Anomalies

  • Of the posterior default mode
  • In the ventral striatum

during error handling.

Executive function deficits also show in other studies62636465

Underfunction in

  • Frontal gyrus
    • Pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA)
    • Middle frontal gyrus
    • Precentral frontal gyrus
  • Insula
  • Caudates
  • Thalamus

Hyperfunction in the

  • Inferior frontal gyrus
  • Postcentral gyrus
  • Precuneus

1.3.2. ADHD-C with inhibition deficits and reward deficits

Overactivation in

  • Mostly non-overlapping cortical and subcortical regions

during error processing and

  • Overactivated amygdala regions and
  • Overactivated ventral striatum regions,

when they made an effort to receive rewards.

1.3.3. ADHD-C without reward deficits and without inhibition deficits

This was the most common subgroup. No neurological abnormalities were found, corresponding to the absence of the specific symptoms mentioned above

1.4. Further subtype classifications

We do not consider the following “subtypes”, which have occasionally been mentioned in the literature, to be genuine presentations (subtypes) of ADHD.

1.4.1. ADHD-RI: Restrictive inattentive subtype

ADHD-RI was not included as a presentation form in DSM 5 due to a lack of robust neurobiological data.
ADHD-RI differs from groups ADHD-I and ADHD-C by:

  • less externalizing behaviour66
    • possibly due to less impairment of the DMN and/or greater impairment of the salience network66
  • poorer sustained attention66
  • better reaction inhibition66
  • ADHD-RI showed a significantly different neuropsychological profile compared to the other groups, including a
  • lower psychomotor speed67
  • longer response times6768
  • the worst overall performance in the global neurocognitive index6768
    • poorer neurocognitive index than SCT (p < 0.001)68
  • significantly increased proportion with a DRD4-7 repeat allele67
  • attention-related posterior brain regions (especially temporal-occipital areas) activated more strongly for go and no-go cues than for controls and ADHD-I67
  • slower psychomotor speed than SCT68
  • only SCT was independently associated with poorer overall memory performance68

1.4.2. ADHD with oppositional behavior disorder (?)

Oppositional behavior disorder is, in our opinion, a comorbidity that occurs primarily in the ADHD-HI type and not a subtype of ADHD-HI.
Significantly, even among those who assume presentation forms (subtypes) of ADHD with oppositional behavior disorder, this is only described in conjunction with the ADHD-C and ADHD-HI type, but not with the (stress phenotypically introverted) ADHD-I type.

1.4.3. Rage type (?)

The Rage type is rarely described as a separate type. It is probably used to describe ADHD-HI with aggressive comorbidity.

Similarly, a subtype with increased emotional irritability/irritability is proposed.69

1.4.4. Residual type

In some cases, a partially regressed, no longer fully developed but still present ADHD is referred to as the residual type.
This is not a subtype of ADHD, but could explain why there are sometimes ADHD sufferers who lack individual key symptoms (e.g. with a largely full symptom picture without attention problems).

1.4.5. ADHD adult subtype pair according to Reimherr

In 8 replication studies of 1,490 adults with ADHD, Reimherr et al identified two clusters, the inattentive adult subtype and the emotionally dysregulated adult subtype.70. The result is consistent with an earlier study by the authors.71
Both forms of presentation (subtypes) benefited equally from MPH treatment.

1.4.5.1. Inattentive adult subtype
  • Inattentive
  • Emotionally dysregulated
1.4.5.2. Emotionally dysregulated adult subtype

Emotional dysregulation manifests itself, among other things, in

  • Anger
  • Affective instability
  • Emotional overreactivity

1.4.6. ADHD presentation forms (subtypes) according to internalizing / externalizing symptoms

Katsuki et al describe four ADHD subtypes based on a cluster analysis of emotional and behavioral symptoms in children aged 4 to 15 with ADHD:72

  • “Strongly internalizing/externalizing”
    • Overlapping of internalizing and externalizing symptoms
      • Possibly moderated by emotional dysregulation and associated neurophysiological correlates
    • High rate of comorbid autism spectrum disorders
    • Increased autistic characteristics
  • “Inattention and internalization”
    • High rate of predominant inattention (ADHD-I)
  • “Aggression and externalization”
    • High rate of comorbid oppositional defiant behavior
    • High rate of comorbid behavioral disorders
  • “Minor psychopathology”
    • Low values on all syndrome scales

1.4.7. ADHD subtypes according to symptom severity

Volk et al defined seven subtypes.7374 They are presented according to frequency (% in brackets). The frequency of the comorbidities depression, ODD (oppositional defiant disorder) and CD (conduct disorder) within the respective group is also stated.

  • “mild ADHD symptoms” (53.4%)
  • “slight inattention” (12.3 %)
    thereof
    • Comorbid depression 9.3 %
    • Comorbid ODD 7.7 %
    • Comorbid CD 6.2 %
  • “severe inattention” (12.1%)
    • Comorbid depression 6.6 %
    • Comorbid ODD 24.5 %
    • Comorbid CD 10.2 %
  • “mild combined symptoms” (6.6%)
    • Comorbid depression 10.7 %
    • Comorbid ODD 30.2 %
    • Comorbid CD 13.6 %
  • “severe combined symptoms” (6.1%)
    • Comorbid depression 8.3 %
    • Comorbid ODD 56.6 %
    • Comorbid CD 12.5 %
  • “talkative and impulsive” (6, 5 %)
    • Comorbid depression 1.9 %
    • Comorbid ODD 24 %
    • Comorbid CD 4.9 %
  • “Hyperactive” (3 %)
    • Comorbid depression 4.4 %
    • Comorbid ODD 16.7 %
    • Comorbid CD 2.2 %

1.4.8. Subtypes according to emotion profiles

One study divided ADHD into three subtypes based on character traits and emotional profiles:69

  • Mild ADHD subtype
    • Normal emotional functionality was found in this group of ADHD sufferers.
  • Distressed ADHD subtype
    • This subtype was characterized by a particularly high level of hardship.
  • Irritable ADHD subtype
    • This subtype showed high negative affect and had the highest external validity.
    • Increased susceptibility to anger
    • The subtype was moderately stable over time and improved prospective prediction of clinical outcomes beyond standard baseline indicators.
    • De subtype was not reducible to ADHD-HI + Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD), ADHD-HI + disruptive mood dysregulation disorder, or other patterns of comorbidity.

1.4.9. Subtypes according to microcognition biomarkers

The symptom-based diagnosis does not match the underlying neuropathology, which complicates the development of new therapies and treatment selection for individual patients. Fine-grained, cost-effective, non-invasive and scalable digital microcognition biomarkers could identify patients with the same symptom-based diagnosis but different neuropathology.
A study of n = 69 children aged 6 to 9 years with ADHD compared performance variables from a Go/NoGo test with n = 58 typically developing (TD) children and identified four subgroups using microcognitive biomarkers identified from thousands of responses during digital neurotherapy:75

  • Cluster 4:
    • poor reaction inhibition
    • inconsistent attention
    • much greater ability to recognize natural categories, which children learn through physical interaction with the environment, than members of abstract categories
  • Cluster 3
    • poor reaction inhibition
  • Cluster 2
    • faster and more consistent reactions than TD
    • better detection of simple targets than TD
    • better working memory than TD
    • Attention problems; significant loss of performance with
      • Pursuit of multiple goals (divided attention)
      • Distraction
  • Cluster 1
    • much greater ability to recognize members of abstract categories than natural categories that children learn through physical interaction with the environment

2. Neurophysiological and endocrine differences between the subtypes

Hyperactivity is neurologically anchored in the striatum, inattention neurologically primarily in the PFC.76

A further distinction may need to be made between inattention due to boredom caused by an under-activated PFC (in ADHD-I) and distractibility due to over-activation of the PFC (in ADHD-HI).

2.1. Dopamine level

This section deals with the question of whether different forms of presentation (subtypes) (according to symptom severity) correlate with certain dopamine (effect) levels. The question of whether dopamine deficiency and dopamine excess are possibly different disorders or at least variants of ADHD that need to be distinguished from each other is dealt with at the end of this chapter.

2.1.1. Hyperactivity, impulsivity: dopamine deficiency or excess dopamine in the striatum

Hyperactivity and impulsivity, as exhibited by the ADHD-HI subtype (without inattention) or ADHD-C (with inattention), are primarily caused (in relation to ADHD) by deviations of the dopamine level from the optimal dopamine level in the right hemispheric striatum (involving the frontostriatal loop, consisting of the PFC, caudate nucleus and globus pallidus, but not the putamen).767778

The prevailing opinion in the specialist literature is that ADHD is caused by a dopamine deficiency. However, it is known from animal models (e.g. the DAT-KO mouse) that an excess of dopamine in the striatum can also trigger hyperactivity. For more information, see ADHD in the animal model in the chapter Neurological aspects. As long as ADHD is not neurobiologically defined (and diagnosed) as a dopamine deficit in the striatum (and/or PFC), but is diagnosed solely on the basis of symptoms, both variants must be taken into consideration. According to the inverted-U model7980 , both excess and deficiency of neurotransmitters cause almost identical symptoms, as the functionality of signal transmission is only given at optimal neurotransmitter levels. Nevertheless, the evidence for a lack of phasic dopamine in the striatum in ADHD is far outnumbered.

Since dopamine degradation in the striatum is mainly via DAT, while dopamine degradation in the PFC is mainly via NET and COMT and not via DAT, the DAT-10R gene variant correlated strongly with hyperactivity and impulsivity, but not with inattention.8176
Another study found no difference in DAT 9/9, 9/10 or 10/10 gene variants (or in DRD4 gene variants) between ADHD-I on the one hand and ADHD-C and ADHD-HI on the other.36

2.1.2. Hyperactivity, impulsivity: excess dopamine in the PFC

If the prevailing view in the specialist literature is that hyperactivity and impulsivity (in ADHD-HI and ADHD-C) are caused by a lack of dopamine in the striatum,82 the dopamine seesaw between the striatum and PFC leads to increased dopamine levels in the PFC in hyperactivity and impulsivity (in ADHD-HI and ADHD-C). For more information, see The dopamine seesaw between the PFC and subcortical regions (including the striatum) In the article Dopamine in the chapter Neurological aspects

In healthy people, a high dopamine level in the PFC appears to lead to a low dopamine level in the striatum, and vice versa.
A fully utilized PFC (high dopamine level) simply does not seem to need any stimulation from the reinforcement/motivation center and therefore signals to it: “closed due to overcrowding - give it a rest”. The striatum then sulks quietly (underactivated = low in dopamine). This is a normal, healthy control loop.
A long-lasting tonic dopamine deficiency in the striatum leads to an upregulation of the D2 autoreceptors (due to a very long-lasting excess of dopamine in the PFC), which in turn leads to an upregulation of the dopamine transporters in the striatum. See also Up- and downregulation of the dopamine transporter In the article Dopamine in the chapter Neurological aspects. This could explain an overactivity of the DAT, which would help explain or reinforce the tonic dopamine deficiency in the striatum assumed in ADHD. .

Since hyperactivity can be mediated by a lack of phasic dopamine in the striatum, it would be understandable from this why this only occurs in ADHD-HI and ADHD-C, which are said to have a permanent overactivation of the PFC. The ADHD-I-typical underactivation of the PFC should rather cause an excess of dopamine in the striatum due to the associated dopamine deficiency in the PFC. Studies confirm a dopamine deficiency in the striatum in ADHD, but do not differentiate between the forms of presentation (subtypes).82

In the ADHD literature, it is often argued that too many / overactivated dopamine transporters (DAT) are present in ADHD. If in ADHD-HI a long-lasting excess of dopamine in the PFC causes a long-lasting dopamine deficiency, which in turn triggers upregulation of the dopamine transporters - just as excess dopamine due to amphetamine abuse probably triggers downregulation83 - the dopamine transporters in ADHD-I should consequently be less strongly increased / overactive than in ADHD-HI. Surprisingly, there are only a few studies on the number / activity of DAT in the different ADHD subtypes. These have so far tended to show that ADHD-HI is much more strongly associated with an increased number of DAT than ADHD-I.

2.1.3. DAT differences in the forms of presentation (subtypes)

Various studies indicate an increased number of DAT in ADHD-HI compared to ADHD-I.

  • In a SPECT examination of 31 adults with ADHD, a greater increase in DAT was found in ADHD-HI sufferers than in ADHD-I sufferers. However, DAT were still elevated in ADHD-I sufferers compared to non-affected individuals. Smoking significantly reduced DAT to or below the level of non-affected individuals in both forms of presentation.84 One study compared DAT in the Spontanuous(ly) hypertensive rat (SHR), which is a validated model of the ADHD-C subtype, and a substrain of the Wistar Kyoto rat, which was used here as a model of the purely inattentive ADHD-I subtype. The ADHD-HI subtype rat formed more DAT than the ADHD-I subtype rat, supporting our conclusion. MPH decreased DAT density more in ADHD-HI rats than in ADHD-I rats.85
  • Another study compared rats considered to be ADHD-HI models, ADHD-I models and unaffected models. It found that ADHD-HI rats had significantly lower levels of dopamine in the dorsal striatum, while in ADHD-I rats this was sometimes the same and sometimes even higher than in unaffected rats. Similarly, ADHD-HI rats had faster dopamine uptake in the ventral striatum and nucleus accumbens, while ADHD-I rats had faster dopamine uptake only in the nucleus accumbens, both compared to unaffected rats.86 Another study also found lower dopamine levels (and slightly higher norepinephrine levels) in the striatum in ADHD-HI rats compared to unaffected rats.87 This suggests increased DAT in ADHD-HI compared to ADHD-I.
  • MPH, which significantly reduces the number of DAT, is said to be nowhere near as effective in ADHD-I as in ADHD-HI, according to one study.88 Another study reports a good effect of MPH in ADHD-I.89
  • SCT sufferers (we had previously considered SCT to be an extreme form or subtype of ADHD-I) are particularly frequent MPH nonresponders. In particular, elevated SCT sluggish/sleepy factor values indicate MPH nonresponding. Neither elevated SCT daydreamy symptoms, nor ADHD subtype (ADHD-HI or ADHD-I) differed in MPH responding rates. The latter supports that SCT is not a subtype of ADHD-I.25
  • Genetic differences between the presentation forms (subtypes) in relation to the ADHD-associated polymorphisms of the DAT1 gene are thought to suggest an association between DAT1 gene polymorphisms and ADHD-HI, but not with ADHD-I. The DAT-10R gene variant correlated strongly with hyperactivity and impulsivity, but not with inattention.9076 However, another study was unable to confirm this.91 Another study found an association of DAT1 10/10 with ADHD-I, with DAT1 10/10 being associated with increased DAT expression and reduced dopamine uptake. The results of this study do not match any of the previously known results in other respects either.92 Another study found that
    Another study found no difference in DAT 9/9, 9/10 or 10/10 gene variants (or in DRD4 gene variants) between ADHD-I on the one hand and ADHD-C and ADHD-HI on the other.36
    COMT Val/Val and DAT 10R in combination correlated with increased hyperactivity and increased ADHD symptoms at age 18 in 11- to 15-year-old boys, but not in girls93

DAT 10R stands for more active DAT.94

However, it is questionable whether DAT actually regulates dopamine levels in the way previously assumed.83

2.2. Exaggerated and flattened endocrine stress responses of the presentation forms (subtypes)

The endocrine stress responses appear to be much stronger in ADHD-I than in ADHD-HI and ADHD-C.
An endocrine stress response is the amount of hormones (and neurotransmitters) released in response to an acute stressor.
The ADHD-I subtype often shows an exaggerated cortisol stress response, while the ADHD-HI and ADHD-C often correlate with a flattened cortisol stress response. Thus, the ADHD-I subtype is typically a case of hypercortisolism, while ADHD-HI and ADHD-C are more a case of hypocortisolism.
Changes in cortisol levels in ADHD in: Cortisol and other stress hormones in ADHD

As hypocortisolism can be a long-term follow-up reaction to hypercortisolism, if a downregulation adjustment (⇒ downregulation / upregulation) is necessary due to permanently excessive cortisol levels Downregulation / Upregulation; ⇒ Changed hormone and neurotransmitter levels depending on the stress phase in the article ⇒ The human stress system - the basics of stress In the chapter Stress) of the glucocorticoid (cortisol) receptor systems, it could be concluded that the ADHD-I type represents a precursor and the ADHD-HI and ADHD-C a subsequent stage of ADHD. However, this is contradicted by the fact that the type of cortisol response to acute stress tends to reflect a stress phenotype that is already found in healthy individuals. In disorders with externalizing symptoms - aggression, ODD, CD, etc. - the cortisol responses to acute stress are regularly flattened, while in disorders with internalizing symptoms - depression, anxiety, etc. - the cortisol responses are regularly elevated.

Increases in cortisol are associated with the stress-induced release of noradrenaline and α1-adrenergic receptor activation.9596 Parallel to the issue of the correlation of neurotransmitter noradrenaline and cortisol discussed here, there is a correlation between cortisol and dopamine release.97 Cortisol levels correlate positively with AMP-induced dopamine release in the left ventral striatum and dorsal putamen.

We assume that the cortisol, adrenaline, noradrenaline and dopamine responses in the brain to acute stress correlate, so that a high cortisol response to acute stress is accompanied by a high (hormone) noradrenaline release in the sympathetic nervous system on the one hand and a high (neurotransmitter) noradrenaline and dopamine release in the central nervous system (= brain) on the other. Low values are also likely to correlate. This could conclusively explain several patterns of ADHD subtypes.

Such a correlation was observed for (hormone) noradrenaline in the sympathetic nervous system to cortisol on the basis of alpha-amylase measurements.9899 however, (hormone) noradrenaline from the adrenal medulla only crosses the blood-brain barrier to a small extent and therefore cannot significantly influence the (neurotransmitter) noradrenaline level in the brain.

Our hypothesis is supported by the fact that the HPA axis (which secretes cortisol) and the locus coeruleus (which secretes noradrenaline) are activated by the same instances, which makes a parallelism of the intensity of the reactions conceivable, namely:

  • Mesocortical / mesolimbic system (dopaminergic)
  • Amygdala (serotonergic, acetylcholinergic)
  • Hippocampus (serotonergic, acetylcholinergic)

Details on the hypothesis of a correlation between the cortisol response and the neurotransmitter norepinephrine response to stress

The measurement of (neurotransmitter) noradrenaline is only possible in the spinal fluid (as the body’s hormone noradrenaline produces the same metabolites (degradation products) and can only cross the blood-brain barrier to a limited extent). Cortisol can cross the blood-brain barrier, which is why cortisol can alternatively be measured in blood or saliva.

Results in relation to stress:

  • Stress simultaneously increases (neurotransmitter) noradrenaline and cortisol levels in the brain.1009596 An increase in cortisol is associated with stress, while an increase in noradrenaline is associated with increased arousal.

Results for ADHD:

  • Unfortunately, only one study on (neurotransmitter) noradrenaline in ADHD has been found so far, which also does not take cortisol into account. In the spinal fluid of hyperactive ADHD-HI sufferers, the serotonin metabolite 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (5-HIAA) correlated positively with aggression (unexpected), the dopamine and noradrenaline metabolite homovanillic acid (HVA) positively with hyperactivity and the noradrenaline metabolite 3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylglycol (MHPG) with aggression, delinquency and behavioral problems. The ADHD-I subtype was not included. The results were unexpected even for the authors.101

In the case of psychological disorders or psychological stress, a positive correlation between (neurotransmitter) noradrenaline and cortisol response to the stressor was found almost unanimously. Physical stress (hypoglycaemia), on the other hand, follows a different control pattern.
Nevertheless, the question of a positive, negative or no correlation could be disorder-specific. Only studies of ADHD sufferers will be able to provide certainty, whereby a distinction must be made between forms of presentation (subtypes).

A positive correlation between neurotransmitter norepinephrine and cortisol has been found in various studies:

  • In rhesus monkeys, early stress caused by a 4-day separation from the mother causes an increase in the noradrenaline metabolite MHPG in the cerebrospinal fluid as well as an increase in cortisol in the blood.102
  • The results of an elaborate study of cortisol, noradrenaline and CRH levels in the spinal fluid (which by its nature cannot contain hormonal noradrenaline from the body) found correlating (elevated) cortisol and noradrenaline neurotransmitter levels in depression, with the correlation persisting across circadian changes throughout the day. CRH levels in the spinal fluid, on the other hand, did not correlate with cortisol levels.103
  • A further study with 140 depression sufferers confirms the correlation between the noradrenaline metabolite MHPG in the spinal fluid and blood cortisol on dexamethasone.104
  • In dogs, a correlation between cortisol, noradrenaline, ACTH and β-endorphin in the cerebrospinal fluid and physical exertion was found.105
  • In PTSD, an increase in noradrenaline in the spinal fluid correlates with the severity of the symptoms.106 The cortisol response to acute stress and to dexamethasone is also increased in PTSD.107 In a study (with n = 8, far too small) of PTSD sufferers (war victims), noradrenaline levels in the cerebrospinal fluid increased when watching a traumatizing video, whereas CRH and cortisol blood levels were (unexpectedly for the authors) lower when watching a traumatizing video than when watching a neutral video. ACTH remained unchanged.108 The results of basal cortisol levels in PTSD are contradictory (not increased or decreased).109
  • In monkeys, increased levels of noradrenaline, cortisol and corticotrophin in the cerebrospinal fluid correlate with aggressiveness, while 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid (a metabolite of serotonin) was reduced.110
  • The increased CRH levels in the cerebrospinal fluid in Alzheimer’s disease correlate significantly with increased blood cortisol levels and increased cortisol responses (lack of cortisol suppression in 6 out of 10 subjects) to the dexamethasone test.111
  • Basal cortisol and noradrenaline levels in the cerebrospinal fluid correlate in severe and mild Alzheimer’s disease. Unexpectedly, this correlation was not found in unaffected people.112
  • In the case of fish, specimens that are defeated in fights show a long-lasting increase in cortisol and noradrenaline.113

No positive correlation between neurotransmitter norepinephrine and cortisol was found during physical stress (hypoglycemia):

  • Hypoglycemia (low blood sugar) caused by insulin administration increases cortisol and decreases norepinephrine (as a neurotransmitter) in the cerebrospinal fluid, while norepinephrine levels in the blood (norepinephrine as a body hormone) increase.114

In any case, the fact that cortisol inhibits the locus coeruleus and thus reduces the release of noradrenaline in the CNS does not contradict our understanding of a correlation between cortisol and the neurotransmitter noradrenaline,18 because this merely describes the effect of the cortisol that has already been released, which also inhibits the HPA axis and thus itself. In addition, noradrenaline is released in the sympathetic nervous system and in the brain in response to acute stress before cortisol.

Our assumption is also not contradicted by the fact that dexamethasone only increases the dopamine but not the noradrenaline level in the PFC (and the dopamine and noradrenaline level in the striatum) in ADHD-HI rats, while in unaffected rats the dopamine and noradrenaline levels in the PFC remained unchanged and only dopamine increased in the striatum,87 because a reaction to cortisol is also described here, which is released after the noradrenaline in the PFC during stress.
It should be noted that the cortisol response to dexamethasone is flattened in ADHD-HI (also in SHR rats) compared to non-affected individuals. Nevertheless, dexamethasone has a positive effect on ADHD-HI symptoms in ADHD-HI rats. In the PFC, the effect of dexamethasone on the ADHD-HI phenotype was predominant, while in the striatum the phenotype showed stronger influences than the medication.

The hypothesis is also not contradicted by the results of studies according to which cortisol and catecholamineblood levels Do not form a correlation.115 Catecholamines in the blood only reflect the body’s hormonal noradrenaline levels, but not the levels of neurotransmitters in the CNS, which can be separated from each other due to the blood-brain barrier. Measurements of vanillin mandelic acid in urine, which is a metabolite of noradrenaline in the brain, would be necessary.116

Independently of this, measurements of cortisol and alpha-amylase in response to acute stress, which are carried out in small intervals (2-minute rhythm), showed that, depending on the person affected, the cortisol maximum occurs up to 14 minutes before or up to 14 minutes after the alpha-amylase maximum, so that a correlation between these two values using individual fixed measurements taken x minutes after the stressor would be misleading.117

Studies found correlations between basal cortisol and vanillic mandelic acid levels (the latter as a noradrenaline neurotransmitter metabolite) in cluster headaches,118 and as responses to stressors such as the dexamethasone test in PTSD,119 depression120121 or post-rape.122
In contrast, no correlation was found between (basal) cortisol blood levels and plasma MPHG as a norepinephrine neurotransmitter metabolite in panic attack sufferers.123 Furthermore, no correlations of basal cortisol and noradrenaline levels were found in a study (with n = 10, far too small).124 When evaluating the measurement of MHPG and vanillin mandelic acid in blood or urine, it should be noted that only 20% of MHPG results from the metabolism of the neurotransmitter noradrenaline in the brain, so that the vast majority of MHPG results from hormone noradrenaline in the body. In addition, more than half of MHPG is converted into vanillinmandelic acid.125126 As a result, the neurotransmitter norepinephrine (in the brain) cannot be measured independently of the hormone norepinephrine (in the body) by blood or urine levels of metabolites.

On the other hand, it was found that the levels of noradrenaline in the cerebrospinal fluid correlate with the levels of (cortisol and) noradrenaline in the blood and noradrenaline metabolites in the urine.104 However, this is a correlation and not an identity.

A very strong increase in noradrenaline / dopamine shuts down the PFC and shifts behavioral control to posterior brain regions.2627282930 There are connections between cortisol and the stimulation of D1 receptors.87
The PFC reacts very sensitively to its neurochemical environment. Too little (drowsiness) or too much (severe stress) catecholamine release in the PFC weakens the cognitive control of behavior and attention.127is the PFC remains permanently activated in ADHD-HI and ADHD-C - which is just too much at some point.

ADHD-I sufferers, on the other hand, suffer from an endocrinological overreaction to acute stress. Assuming the hypothesis put forward on this side that in ADHD-I, in addition to the cortisol stress response, the release of noradrenaline in the brain in response to acute stress is also excessive, the confirmed finding that greatly increased noradrenaline levels block the PFC would explain why ADHD-I sufferers often experience mental blocks and excessive demands when making decisions.
The strong stress cortisol response in ADHD-I, which occurs on the 3rd stage of the HPA axis some time after the noradrenaline release, leads to a strong brake on the release of noradrenaline. The norepinephrine brake of the PFC is therefore released again in ADHD-I and the PFC starts up again after some time. The PFC therefore does not remain permanently in a dysfunctional state (as in ADHD-HI/ADHD-C). A merely temporary hypofunction of the PFC therefore does not lead to a permanent excess of dopamine in the striatum, which is why ADHD-I does not cause such severe striatal problems. (Excess and deficiency both cause disturbances in nerve communication and can therefore trigger different or almost identical symptoms in the same brain region, because the communication of signals in the brain only works properly when the neurotransmitter level is optimal).

The various forms of presentation (subtypes) can - in accordance with the endocrinological / neurological characteristics described - also be conclusively described as stress phenotypes.
Stress phenotypes mean a typified reaction to stress.

More on this below under 6.

The PFC is deactivated by alpha-1 adrenoceptors, which have a lower affinity for noradrenaline and cortisol than alpha-2 adrenoceptors and are therefore only addressed at very high noradrenaline and cortisol levels.

On this side, it is assumed that a strong increase in dopamine/noradrenaline is the cause of the underactivation of the PFC typical of ADHD-I.
Since ADHD-HI (and ADHD-C) involves a flattened cortisol response to an acute stressor, a parallel flattened norepinephrine/dopamine release would lead to a permanent mild stress state that does not shut down the PFC but permanently overactivates it.

Due to a weaker cortisol response to acute stress, the cortisol level at the end of a stress reaction in ADHD-HI and ADHD-C is in any case (and regardless of the hypothesis of a correlation of the amounts released on this side) less able to shut down the stress systems of the HPA axis and the release of noradrenaline in the brain.

2.3. ADHD subtypes according to EEG

Most examinations find different EEG subtypes in ADHD sufferers.
Externalizing characters show a flattened error-related negativity (ERN), internalizing characters an increased ERN.128 The ERN is a component of the event-related potentials (qEEG). It occurs immediately after an incorrect motor response (given under time pressure). ERN is primarily measured via the PFC.
An attempt to automatically differentiate subtypes based on their EEG patterns using AI failed.5

The largest study known to us differentiated between 5 EEG subtypes and showed that externalizing symptoms (ADHD-HI, ADHD-C) were primarily associated with increased slow EEG activity in the theta and delta bands, while internalizing symptoms (ADHD-I) were associated with increased activity in the alpha and beta bands.129 In contrast, a study of 94 boys between 6 and 9 years of age found uniformly increased total alpha activity with reduced alpha peak frequency, reduced alpha bandwidth and reduced alpha amplitude suppression magnitude as well as an increased alpha1 / alpha2 (a1 / a2) ratio, despite differentiation by subtype.130

2.3.1. ADHD-HI subtypes according to EEG

EEG abnormalities in ADHD-HI are primarily found in the striatum and in the frontal-striatal area.131

2.3.1.1. Theta frontal increased, beta decreased (ADHD-HI with hyperactivity and impulsivity with hypoactive EEG)

A high theta-beta ratio defines a separate EEG ADHD subtype.132 This subtype was described in 24.5% of ADHD sufferers.133

  • Total power increased overall133
  • Relative theta increased, especially frontal133
    • Increased theta frontal was found in several EEG studies in ADHD134
    • This is considered representative of frontal lobe problems, especially hyperactivity135
  • Relative and absolute theta increased136
  • Beta activity reduced overall133
    • Lower amplitude at relative beta, measured at the electrode derivation points Cz on Pz-O1 and Pz-O2 as well as paramedian136
  • Consequence: Theta-beta ratio increased (theta high, beta low)137
  • Delta central and posterior reduced133
  • Alpha front and rear reduced133
  • Increased slow waves, reduced fast waves138
  • Type of behavior133
    • Most typical ADHD symptom pattern of all EEG subtypes
    • Hyperactivity
    • Pleasurable
    • Less anxious

Therapy goals:

  • Increase SMR (12 to 15 Hz)
  • Reduce theta (4 to 8 Hz)
  • Reduce theta while increasing beta: addresses tonic aspects of cortical activation to achieve an alert, focused yet calm state139

Effectiveness:

  • Train theta down and (possibly simultaneously) alpha up
    The last two training methods mentioned (theta down, beta up or theta down / alpha up training) differed only slightly in their results for 7-10-year-olds. Both protocols alleviate the symptoms of ADHD-HI in general (p <0.001) as well as the symptoms of hyperactivity (p <0.001), inattention (p <0.001) and omission errors (p <0.001), but not the oppositional and impulsive symptoms.140
  • Of 19 participants with ADHD-HI according to DSM III-R 12, 11 responded very well to neurofeedback training in which beta was trained up and theta down (40 sessions). The other 7 showed less improvement. In the responders, the IQ improved by 10 points (from 112 to 122) in addition to the symptoms. However, N = 19 is too small for a reliable statement.141
2.3.1.2. Excessive beta (“overactivated” ADHD-HI)

The beta subtype is said to be present in around 23.2%142 of ADHD sufferers.

Findings:143144

  • Amplitude of beta (13 to 30 Hz)145 or (18-26 Hz) increased, frontal and at the back of the head
  • Beta dominates relative and absolute power
  • Occurrence in the entire cortex
  • Increased overall frontal performance142
  • Increased beta activity over the entire top of the skull142
  • Theta activity reduced in the fronto-central regions142
  • Alpha activity in the frontal regions reduced142
  • Type of behavior142
    • Symptomatic:
      • Increased aggressiveness
      • Increased vandalism
      • Increased antisocial behavior.
    • Subtype-specific:
      This neurological abnormality is not found in ADHD-I, but only in a subgroup of the mixed type, which is also distinguished from the rest of ADHD-C by a greater tendency to develop other symptoms 146138
      • Tantrums
      • Mood swings
      • Increased delinquency

However, ADHD sufferers with excessive beta are not hyperactive. Compared to non-affected people, this is typical:147

  • Beta increased overall
  • Delta is significantly reduced centrally posteriorly
  • Alpha is reduced overall
  • Significantly reduces the overall posterior performance
  • The theta / beta ratio is reduced overall.
  • The skin conductance is significantly reduced (just as in patients with excessively elevated theta)

From this, the authors conclude that the theta/beta ratio is not associated with arousal.

Therapy goals:

  • Increase SMR (12 to 15 Hz)
  • Reduce Beta 2 (21 to 35 Hz)
  • Reduce gamma (35 to 45 Hz)

A renowned group of researchers reported that ADHD sufferers with very low EEG theta values are more often non-responders with regard to stimulants.148 However, we know of beta-type sufferers who benefited greatly from methylphenidate and even more from amphetamine medication.

2.3.2. ADHD-I subtypes in the EEG

EEG abnormalities in ADHD-I are said to occur primarily in the PFC and in the frontal-paretial area.131

2.3.2.1. Delta and theta increased, beta decreased

One study found this subtype in 24.5% of ADHD sufferers.149 Characteristic is:

  • Delta increased overall136149
  • Total theta increased136149
  • Abnormal increase in slow frequencies (delta/theta) primarily in central and centro-frontal areas150
  • Alpha reduced overall149
  • Beta reduced136
  • Total posterior power reduced149
  • Type of behavior149
    • Enthusiastic
    • Impulsive
    • Requires close supervision to complete tasks
    • Less need for sleep
    • Swears, uses obscene language
    • No disorder of social behavior (Conduct Disorder, CD)
    • Prefers to surround himself with younger children
    • Developmental delay
  • ADHD sufferers with high front-midline theta often showed
    • High and steep power peaks when discharged to Fz
    • With high FMT activity in the higher FMT frequencies:
      • Problems with emotion regulation and memory control.
    • With high FMT activity in the lower FMT frequencies:
      • Learning difficulties or memory problems.
    • States of anxiety
    • Affect breakthroughs

Therapy goals:

  • Increase beta 1 (15 to 21 Hz)
  • Reduce theta (4 to 8 Hz)

Kühle136 describes that delayed brain development is often found in ADHD-I. As far as we know, the maturation of some areas of the brain is delayed in all ADHD subtypes. However, whether this represents a neurological deficit or even just a neurological correlate (= image) of ADHD is questionable. The maturation delay in ADHD sufferers corresponds quite exactly to the brain maturation delay in giftedness.
Find out more at Giftedness and ADHD

2.3.2.2. Only alpha activity increased

This type is said to occur in around 16.8%138 of ADHD sufferers.

  • Excessive alpha activity151138
    • Posterior152
    • Central153
    • Fronto-central138
    • Sometimes also head-on151
  • Delta reduced overall138
  • Theta fronto-central reduced138
  • Type of behavior138
    • Hyperactive, fidgety
    • Confused, feels like in a fog
    • Slightly obsessive
    • Perfectionist, sets high goals
    • Only deals with one or two ideas or activities

The type of behavior described above, which is reminiscent of compulsiveness, is said to be reflected in studies of people with autism, in which very high alpha values were also found.154155

The alpha activity occurs as a μ-rhythm (also called monkey face (Mu-rhythm)) centrally over the entire cortex in the posterior temporal and/or temporal area.

A study of girls with ADHD-I only found significant differences in the alpha 2 band compared to non-affected girls.156

2.3.2.3. Theta posterior increased, alpha and beta decreased

This type is said to occur in around 11%138 of ADHD sufferers.

  • Total frontal power significantly increased147
  • Theta significantly increased147
    • Especially posterior
  • Theta / beta ratio significantly increased147
  • Alpha reduced across the entire skullcap147
  • Beta reduced across the entire skullcap147
  • Delta activity reduced centrally.147
  • Type of behavior147
    • Outgoing
    • Cheerful
    • Fewer behavioral problems than other ADHD sufferers
    • Significantly less hyperactivity
    • Age-appropriate behavior (no developmental delay)
    • No increased anxiety
    • No increased antisocial behavior
    • No ritualized behavior
2.3.2.4. Only theta frontal midline (FMT) increased
  • Abnormal increase in frontal midline theta (FMT) (5.5 to 8 Hz),157158 especially under task load
  • Dysfunction: hippocampus, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC)159
  • Medication recommendation: No medication; low doses of methylphenidate159
  • Unaffected people have hardly any FMT at rest
  • ADHD sufferers with high FMT often show
    • High and steep power peaks when discharged to Fz
    • With high FMT activity in the higher FMT frequencies:
      • Frequent problems with emotion regulation and memory control
    • With high FMT activity in the lower FMT frequencies:
      • Often learning difficulties or memory problems
    • States of anxiety
    • Affect breakthroughs

2.4. ADHD subtypes according to QEEG

A study with 69 participants found three QEEG subtypes:160

  • increased absolute and relative beta performance
    • K-ARS: 25.31
  • increased relative fast alpha and beta performance
    • K-ARS: 21.67
  • increased absolute slow frequency (delta and theta power)
    • K-ARS: 12.64 and thus hardly more than non-affected persons with 11.07
    • WURS: 55.82 significantly higher than non-affected persons with 42.81

The third group can therefore only be identified with the WURS (Wender-Utah Rating Scale) and not with the K-ARS (Koranic ADHD Rating Scale, the most widely used scale in Korea).

One study found:161

  • an age-dependent decrease in qEEG power in children with ADHD
  • significant differences between children with ADHD and non-affected children in the theta/beta ratio and theta activity in the frontal area
  • remarkable trend towards an increase in beta activity in the age groups 6-10 years and > 10 years
  • for younger children with ADHD
    • Correlation between qEEG power and hyperactivity
    • Correlation between frontal theta activity and hyperactivity
  • The qEEG power of children with ADHD gradually decreased with increasing age, corresponding to the decrease in symptoms

2.5. Functional differences in nerve conduction pathways in the brain (?)

Studies suggesting specific functional differences in the neural pathways in the brain for ADHD-HI and ADHD-I,162163 suffer from very low case numbers (n < 50), which affects the robustness of the results (for more on this, see: Studies prove - sometimes nothing at all).

2.6. Subtypes and serotonin

Furthermore, a different serotonin level in the different subtypes is discussed, which is thought to be related to the 5-HT 1B receptor in ADHD-I and to the 5-HT 2A/C receptors in ADHD-HI.164

2.7. Neuro-auditory profiles of the subtypes

One study demonstrated differences between ADHD-HI, ADHD-I and controls in the auditory brain regions, the Heschl’s gyrus (HG) and the planum temporale (PT).
ADHD-HI and ADHD-I showed reduced gray matter volumes in the left Heschl’s gyrus, and thus reduced HG/PT ratios in the left hemisphere.
ADHD-HI showed a lower right HG/PT ratio in the right hemisphere, while ADHD-I did not differ from controls.
ADHD-HI showed left-right asynchrony, while ADHD-I and controls showed balanced hemispheric response patterns.165

3. ADHD-HI/ADHD-C and ADHD-I as stress phenotypes

See also Stress theories and stress phenotypes: a possible explanation of ADHD subtypes In the chapter Stress

4. The problem of subdivision into subtypes

One problem with the subdivision into subtypes is that some ADHD sufferers transfer brain activities from affected brain areas to other brain areas, i.e. “misappropriate” brain areas in order to substitute the abilities of the affected brain areas.

This correlates with the fact that a genetic disposition (DAT 10-repeat-allele of the dopamine transporter genotype (40-bp 30 VNTR of DAT, SLC6A3) links a high BAS with a high activity of the ventral striatum, while in other genetic dispositions (DAT 9-repeat-allele) a high BAS does not correlate with a high activity of the striatum.166

Similarly, some polymorphisms of the THP2 and 5-HTTLPR genes show a high positive correlation between high BIS and connectivity between the amygdala and hippocampus, while other polymorphisms of these genes show a high negative correlation between high BIS and connectivity between the amygdala and hippocampus.167

This makes diagnosis by means of questionnaires and tests more difficult.

A more objective determination of the respective ADHD subtype could be made through a more precise anamnesis using EEG or QEEG measurement and stress system reactance using the dexamethasone/ACTH/CRH test.

Assuming our hypothesis that the subtypes of ADHD are defined by the natural disposition of the respective affected person with regard to their stress response type (fight/flight/freeze, whereby fight is understood as a type of the hyperactive-impulsive subtype (ADHD-HI) and freeze as a synonym of the purely inattentive subtype (ADHD-I), according to the FF(F)S model of Connor), a QEEG analysis would require that a statistically valid number of healthy individuals with fight/flight/freeze characteristics be included in the QEEG comparison databases in order to be able to compare the specific activities of the individual brain areas with the respective matching comparison values of healthy individuals.
We are not aware that QEEG databases contain any information on this.

5. ADHD variants according to dopamine level?

Most of the specialist literature links ADHD with a reduced dopamine level. This description may also include the fact that the dopamine level is neutral but the sensitivity of receptors is reduced, or that dopamine is broken down too quickly. As a result, too little dopamine (effect) is present.
Apart from the fact that this description usually does not differentiate more precisely in which brain region the dopamine deficit exists and whether it is a deficit of the tonic or phasic dopamine release or of the basal dopamine level, there is conflicting evidence that an excess of dopamine can also cause ADHD symptoms. See, among others, ADHD in animal models In the chapter Neurological aspects.
This is consistent with the inverted-U model, according to which an excess as well as a deficit of a neurotransmitter can cause confusingly similar symptoms, because optimal signal transmission is dependent on a certain (“mean”) neurotransmitter level. Signal transmission is equally impaired by excessive and reduced neurotransmitter levels.

As long as ADHD is defined and diagnosed purely on the basis of symptoms, this will inevitably lead to sufferers with dopamine deficiency being treated in the same way as sufferers with dopamine excess (even if we assume that the latter are rare or at least less common). Against this background, the question arises as to whether it would not make sense to either

  • To define ADHD neurobiologically as a dopamine (action) deficit and to name all forms (even with similar symptoms) with a dopamine surplus differently,
    or
  • ADHD can be divided neurobiologically into two dopaminergic variants*, the hypodopaminergic variant (dopamine deficiency) and the hyperdopaminergic variant (dopamine excess).

*We have deliberately chosen the term variant in order to reserve the term subtype for the different symptom forms (predominantly hyperactive, predominantly inattentive, ADHD-C).

Contrary to our initial expectations, however, findings on the effect of ADHD drugs on hypodopaminergic and hyperdopaminergic animal models show that the drugs primarily used to date appear to have comparable effects on both variants.
Stimulants act primarily as dopamine reuptake inhibitors and thus increase the dopamine level available in the synaptic cleft. Nevertheless, stimulants reduce hyperactivity even in animal models with dopamine excess, such as the DAT-KO mouse, without reducing extracellular dopamine levels. Atomoxetine, on the other hand, appears to reduce hyperactivity only in animal models with dopamine deficiency, but not in dopamine excess. Cognitive impairments such as inattention and learning deficits appear to be improved by stimulants as well as atomoxetine in dopamine excess.
More on this at ADHD in the animal model In the chapter Neurological aspects.

Even though the medications used to date appear to work equally well in cases of dopamine deficiency and dopamine excess, we thought it would be desirable to distinguish between these different ADHD variants more consciously. The fact that the medications used to date work equally in both cases could also be the result of the fact that the effect of medications has not yet been assessed separately for the two variants. It is quite conceivable that, with appropriate differentiation, it could turn out that individual medications that have so far been devalued due to their lower efficacy (compared to the stimulants primarily used to date) prove to be effective for one of the variants and ineffective for the other - as appears to be the case with atomoxetine in relation to hyperactivity. In this case, the efficacy would have to be re-evaluated in relation to the area of application.

The question of ADHD variants (hypodopaminergic/hyperdopaminergic) is unlikely to gain any practical significance in practice as long as there is no inexpensive, reliable and side-effect-free method of determining the dopamine (effect) level in certain regions of the brain in those affected. However, we would like to see greater consideration of this aspect in the scientific study of ADHD and in an in-depth examination of drug treatment of ADHD, as well as a consistent differentiation of study results according to subtypes.


  1. Desman, Petermann (2005): Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit-/Hyperaktivitätsstörung (ADHS): Wie valide sind die Subtypen? Kindheit Und Entwicklung, 14(4), 244–254. doi:10.1026/0942-5403.14.4.244

  2. Millstein, Wilens, Biederman, Spencer (1997): Presenting ADHD symptoms and subtypes in clinically referred adults with ADHD. JOURNAL OF ATTENTION DISORDERS, VOL. 2, NO. 3 (OCTOBER 1997), 159-166. n = 149

  3. Wilens, Biederman, Faraone, Martelon, Westerberg, Spencer (2009): Presenting ADHD symptoms, subtypes, and comorbid disorders in clinically referred adults with ADHD. J Clin Psychiatry. 2009 Nov;70(11):1557-62. doi: 10.4088/JCP.08m04785pur. PMID: 20031097; PMCID: PMC2948439. n = 107

  4. Lahey BB, Pelham WE, Loney J, Lee SS, Willcutt E (2005): Instability of the DSM-IV Subtypes of ADHD from preschool through elementary school. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005 Aug;62(8):896-902. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.8.896. PMID: 16061767.

  5. Vahid, Bluschke, Roessner, Stober, Beste (2019): Deep Learning Based on Event-Related EEG Differentiates Children with ADHD from Healthy Controls. J Clin Med. 2019 Jul 19;8(7). pii: E1055. doi: 10.3390/jcm8071055.

  6. http://www.adhs.info/fuer-paedagogen/speziell-elementarbereich/subtypen.html

  7. http://www.adhs.info/fuer-paedagogen/speziell-primarbereich/subtypen.html

  8. Diamond: Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): A neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit (with hyperactivity), Development and Psychopathology 17 (2005), 807–825, Seite 809

  9. Steinhausen, Rothenberger, Döpfner (2010): Handbuch ADHS, Seite 37

  10. Diamond: Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): A neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit (with hyperactivity), Development and Psychopathology 17 (2005), 807–825, Seite 819

  11. Diamond: Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): A neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit (with hyperactivity), Development and Psychopathology 17 (2005), 807–825, Seite 809

  12. Diamond: Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): A neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit (with hyperactivity), Development and Psychopathology 17 (2005), 807–825, Seiten 810, 813, mit ausführlicher Darlegung

  13. Diamond: Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): A neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit (with hyperactivity), Development and Psychopathology 17 (2005), 807–825, Seiten 810, 811, 812 mit etlichen weiteren Nachweisen

  14. Diamond: Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): A neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit (with hyperactivity), Development and Psychopathology 17 (2005), 807–825, Seite 816

  15. Diamond: Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): A neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit (with hyperactivity), Development and Psychopathology 17 (2005), 807–825, Seite 810

  16. Diamond: Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): A neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit (with hyperactivity), Development and Psychopathology 17 (2005), 807–825, Seite 810

  17. Diamond: Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): A neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit (with hyperactivity), Development and Psychopathology 17 (2005), 807–825, Seite 812 mit weiteren Nachweisen

  18. Pütz (2008): Stress-Erkrankungen: Dysregulationen der Hypothalamus-Hypophysen-Nebennierenrinden-Achse

  19. Kirschbaum (2001): Das Stresshormon Cortisol – Ein Bindeglied zwischen Psyche und Soma? in: Jahrbuch der Heinrich-Heine-Universität Düsseldorf, 2001, 150-156, mwNw

  20. Wagner, Born: Psychoendokrine Aspekte neurophysiologischer Funktionen. In: Lautenbacher, Gauggel (2013): Neuropsychologie psychischer Störungen, Seite 131

  21. Konrad, Herpertz-Dahlmann (2010): Neuropsychologie der Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit/Hyperaktivitätsstörung (ADHD) in: Lautenbach, Gauggel: Neuropsychologie psychischer Störungen, 2. Auflage 2010, Seiten 453 – 472, S. 468, ohne Quellenangabe

  22. Diamond: Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): A neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit (with hyperactivity), Development and Psychopathology 17 (2005), 807–825, Seite 809 mit etlichen weiteren Nachweisen

  23. www.adhspedia.de: Hypoaktivität

  24. Diamond: Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): A neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit (with hyperactivity), Development and Psychopathology 17 (2005), 807–825, Seite 809 mit weiteren Nachweisen

  25. Froehlich, Becker, Nick, Brinkman, Stein, Peugh, Epstein (2018): Sluggish Cognitive Tempo as a Possible Predictor of Methylphenidate Response in Children With ADHD: A Randomized Controlled Trial. J Clin Psychiatry. 2018 Feb 27;79(2). pii: 17m11553. doi: 10.4088/JCP.17m11553.

  26. Ramos, Arnsten (2007): Adrenergic pharmacology and cognition: focus on the prefrontal cortex. Pharmacol Ther. 2007 Mar; 113(3):523-36., Kapitel 6

  27. Birnbaum, Gobeske, Auerbach, Taylor, Arnsten (1999): A role for norepinephrine in stress-induced cognitive deficits: α-1-adrenoceptor mediation in prefrontal cortex. Biol. Psychiatry 46, 1266–1274.

  28. Ramos, Colgan, Nou, Ovadia, Wilson, Arnsten (2005). The beta-1 adrenergic antagonist, betaxolol, improves working memory performance in rats and monkeys. Biol. Psychiatry 58, 894–900.

  29. ähnlich: Arnsten (2000): Stress impairs prefrontal cortical function in rats and monkeys: role of dopamine D1 and norepinephrine alpha-1 receptor mechanisms. Prog Brain Res. 2000;126:183-92.

  30. Für starke Stimulation des D1-Dopaminrezeptors: Zahrt, Taylor, Mathew, Arnsten (1997): Supranormal stimulation of D1 dopamine receptors in the rodent prefrontal cortex impairs spatial working memory performance. J Neurosci. 1997 Nov 1;17(21):8528-35.

  31. Lee, Shin, Stein (2010): Increased cortisol after stress is associated with variability in response time in ADHD children. Yonsei Med J 51:206–211

  32. Diamond: Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): A neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit (with hyperactivity), Development and Psychopathology 17 (2005), 807–825,

  33. Noble, Ozkaragoz, Ritchie, Zhang, Belin, Sparkes (1998): D2 and D4 Dopamine Receptor Polymorphisms and Personality; American Journal of Medical Genetics (Neuropsychiatric Genetics) 81:257–267 (1998); n = 119

  34. Edel, Vollmöller (2006): Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit-/Hyperktivitätsstörung bei Erwachsenen, Seite 104

  35. Simchen, Helga: http://helga-simchen.info/Thesen-zu-ADS, dort unter 8.

  36. Grizenko N, Paci M, Joober R (2010): Is the inattentive subtype of ADHD different from the combined/hyperactive subtype? J Atten Disord. 2010 May;13(6):649-57. doi: 10.1177/1087054709347200. PMID: 19767592.

  37. Curran S, Purcell S, Craig I, Asherson P, Sham P (2005): The serotonin transporter gene as a QTL for ADHD. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2005 Apr 5;134B(1):42-7. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30118. PMID: 15719397.

  38. Grevet EH, Marques FZ, Salgado CA, Fischer AG, Kalil KL, Victor MM, Garcia CR, Sousa NO, Belmonte-de-Abreu P, Bau CH (2007): Serotonin transporter gene polymorphism and the phenotypic heterogeneity of adult ADHD. J Neural Transm (Vienna). 2007;114(12):1631-6. doi: 10.1007/s00702-007-0797-2. PMID: 17690945.

  39. Baykal, Albayrak, Durankuş, Güzel, Abbak, Potas, Beyazyüz, Karabekiroğlu, Donma (2019): Decreased serum orexin A levels in drug-naive children with attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder. Neurol Sci. 2019 Jan 7. doi: 10.1007/s10072-018-3692-8. n = 96

  40. Diamond: Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): A neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit (with hyperactivity), Development and Psychopathology 17 (2005), 807–825, Seite 809 f

  41. Diamond: Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): A neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit (with hyperactivity), Development and Psychopathology 17 (2005), 807–825, Seite 809, mit etlichen weiteren Nachweisen

  42. Diamond: Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): A neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit (with hyperactivity), Development and Psychopathology 17 (2005), 807–825, Seite 812

  43. Diamond: Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): A neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit (with hyperactivity), Development and Psychopathology 17 (2005), 807–825, Seite 811, mit etlichen weiteren Nachweisen

  44. Steinhausen, Rothenberger, Döpfner (2010): Handbuch ADHS, Seite 25

  45. Barkley (1997): Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 65-94.http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65

  46. http://www.adhspedia.de/wiki/Subtypen#Vorwiegend_Hyperaktiv-Impulsives_Erscheinungsbild_der_ADHS

  47. Heim, Ehlert, Hellhammer (2000): The potential role of hypocortisolism in the pathophysiology of stress-related bodily disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 25(1), 1-35

  48. verringerte Stressantwort der HPA-Achse bei Fibromyalgie: Wingenfeld (2003): Eine Untersuchung endokrinen und psychologischen Veränderungen bei PTSD und stressabhängigen körperlichen Beschwerden, Dissertation

  49. erniedrigte Stressantwort der HPA-Achse bei PTBS: Wingenfeld (2003): Eine Untersuchung endokrinen und psychologischen Veränderungen bei PTSD und stressabhängigen körperlichen Beschwerden, Dissertation

  50. Barkley (1997): Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 65-94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65

  51. Arnsten, Contant (1992): a-2 adrenergic agonists decrease distractibility in aged monkeys performing the delayed response task. Psychopharmacology 108, 159-169.

  52. Arnsten, Leslie (1991): Behavioral and receptor binding analysis of the a-2 adrenergic agonist, UK-14304 (5 bromo-6 2-imidazoline-2-yl amino quinoxaline): Evidence for cognitive enhancement at an a-2 adrenoceptor subtype. Neuropharmacology 30, 1279-1289.

  53. Arnsten, Cai, Goldman-Rakic (1988): The a-2 adrenergic agonist guanfacine improves memory in aged monkeys without sedative or hypotensive side effects: Evidence for a-2 receptor subtypes. J. Neurosci. 8, 4287-4298

  54. Cai, Ma, Xu, Hu, (1993): Resperine impairs spatial working memory performance in monkeys: Reversal by the a-2 adrenergic agonist clonidine. Brain Res. 614, 191-196

  55. für Noradrenalin aus dem Nebennierenmark – möglicherweise nur eine Koinzidenz: Skosnik, Chatterton, Swisher, Park (2000): Modulation of attentional inhibition by norepinephrine and cortisol after psychological stress; International Journal of Psychophysiology 36 2000 59-68

  56. Al-Amin, Zinchenko, Geyer (2018): Hippocampal subfield volume changes in subtypes of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, Brain Research, Volume 1685, 2018, Pages 1-8, ISSN 0006-8993, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2018.02.007. n = 880

  57. Fernández-Lópe, Molina-Carballo, Cubero-Millán, Checa-Ros, Machado-Casas, Blanca-Jover, Jerez-Calero, Madrid-Fernández, Uberos, Muñoz-Hoyos (2020): Indole Tryptophan Metabolism and Cytokine S100B in Children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Daily Fluctuations, Responses to Methylphenidate, and Interrelationship with Depressive Symptomatology. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2020 Feb 12. doi: 10.1089/cap.2019.0072. PMID: 32048862.

  58. Adriani, Caprioli, Granstrem, Carli, Laviola (2003): The spontaneously hypertensive-rat as an animal model of ADHD: evidence for impulsive and non-impulsive subpopulations. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2003 Nov;27(7):639-51. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.08.007. PMID: 14624808.

  59. Chen G, Gao W, Xu Y, Chen H, Cai H (2023): Serum TSH Levels are Associated with Hyperactivity Behaviors in Children with Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2023 Mar 7;19:557-564. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S402530. PMID: 36915908; PMCID: PMC10007977.

  60. Stevens, Pearlson, Calhoun, Bessette (2018): Functional Neuroimaging Evidence for Distinct Neurobiological Pathways in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2018 Aug;3(8):675-685. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.09.005.

  61. McCabe LE, Johnstone SJ, Jiang H, Sun L, Zhang DW (2023): Links Between Excessive Daytime Sleepiness and EEG Power and Activation in Two Subtypes of ADHD. Biol Psychol. 2023 Jan 18:108504. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2023.108504. PMID: 36681294.

  62. Lei, Du, Wu, Chen, Huang, Du, Bi, Kemp, Gong (2015): Functional MRI reveals different response inhibition between adults and children with ADHD. Neuropsychology. 2015 Nov;29(6):874-81. doi: 10.1037/neu0000200.

  63. Hart, Radua, Nakao, Mataix-Cols, Rubia (2013): Meta-analysis of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Studies of Inhibition and Attention in Attention-deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Exploring Task-Specific, Stimulant Medication, and Age Effects. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70(2):185–198. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.277

  64. Rae, Hughes, Weaver, Anderson, Rowe (2014): Selection and stopping in voluntary action: A meta-analysis and combined fMRI study, NeuroImage, Volume 86, 2014, Pages 381-391, ISSN 1053-8119, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.012.

  65. Dambacher, Sack, Lobbestael, Arntz, Brugman, Schuhmann (2014): A network approach to response inhibition: dissociating functional connectivity of neural components involved in action restraint and action cancellation. Eur J Neurosci, 39: 821-831. doi:10.1111/ejn.12425

  66. Wu ZM, Wang P, Liu J, Liu L, Cao XL, Sun L, Yang L, Cao QJ, Wang YF, Yang BR (2023): The clinical, neuropsychological, and brain functional characteristics of the ADHD restrictive inattentive presentation. Front Psychiatry. 2023 Mar 1;14:1099882. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1099882. PMID: 36937718; PMCID: PMC10014598. n = 789

  67. Ercan ES, Suren S, Bacanlı A, Yazici KU, Callı C, Ozyurt O, Aygunes D, Kosova B, Franco AR, Rohde LA (2016): Decreasing ADHD phenotypic heterogeneity: searching for neurobiological underpinnings of the restrictive inattentive phenotype. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2016 Mar;25(3):273-82. doi: 10.1007/s00787-015-0731-3. PMID: 26058607. n = 301

  68. Ünsel-Bolat G, Ercan ES, Bolat H, Süren S, Bacanlı A, Yazıcı KU, Rohde LA (2023): Comparisons between sluggish cognitive tempo and ADHD-restrictive inattentive presentation phenotypes in a clinical ADHD sample. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord. 2019 Dec;11(4):363-372. doi: 10.1007/s12402-019-00301-y. PMID: 30911899. n = 314

  69. Karalunas, Gustafsson, Fair, Musser, Nigg (2018): Do we need an irritable subtype of ADHD? Replication and extension of a promising temperament profile approach to ADHD subtyping. Psychol Assess. 2018 Oct 25. doi: 10.1037/pas0000664.

  70. Reimherr, Roesler, Marchant, Gift, Retz, Philipp-Wiegmann, Reimherr (2020): Types of Adult Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Replication Analysis. J Clin Psychiatry. 2020 Mar 17;81(2):19m13077. doi: 10.4088/JCP.19m13077. PMID: 32220152. n = 1.490

  71. Reimherr, Marchant, Gift, Steans, Wender (2015): Types of adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): baseline characteristics, initial response, and long-term response to treatment with methylphenidate. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord. 2015 Jun;7(2):115-28. doi: 10.1007/s12402-015-0176-z. PMID: 25987323.

  72. Katsuki, Yamashita, Yamane, Kanba, Yoshida (2020): Clinical Subtypes in Children with Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder According to Their Child Behavior Checklist Profile. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2020 Dec;51(6):969-977. doi: 10.1007/s10578-020-00977-8. PMID: 32166459. n = 314

  73. Volk, Neuman, Todd (2005): A systematic evaluation of ADHD and comorbid psychopathology in a population-based twin sample. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005 Aug;44(8):768-75. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000166173.72815.83. PMID: 16034278. n = 1.616 REVIEW

  74. Volk, Henderson, Neuman, Todd (2006): Validation of population-based ADHD subtypes and identification of three clinically impaired subtypes. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2006 Apr 5;141B(3):312-8. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30299. PMID: 16526027. n = 1.346

  75. Wexler BE, Kish R . Using micro-cognition biomarkers of neurosystem dysfunction to redefine ADHD subtypes: A scalable digital path to diagnosis based on brain function. Psychiatry Res. 2023 Aug;326:115348. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2023.115348. PMID: 37494880.

  76. Diamond (2011): Biological and social influences on cognitive control processes dependent on prefrontal cortex. Prog Brain Res. 2011;189:319-39. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53884-0.00032-4. PMID: 21489397; PMCID: PMC4103914. REVIEW

  77. Soliva, Fauquet, Bielsa, Rovira, Carmona, Ramos-Quiroga, Hilferty, Bulbena, Casas, Vilarroya (3020): Quantitative MR analysis of caudate abnormalities in pediatric ADHD: proposal for a diagnostic test. Psychiatry Res. 2010 Jun 30;182(3):238-43. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.01.013. PMID: 20488672.

  78. Casey, Castellanos, Giedd, Marsh, Hamburger, Schubert, Vauss, Vaituzis, Dickstein, Sarfatti, Rapoport (1997): Implication of right frontostriatal circuitry in response inhibition and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997 Mar;36(3):374-83. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199703000-00016. PMID: 9055518.

  79. Cools R, D’Esposito M (2011): Inverted-U-shaped dopamine actions on human working memory and cognitive control. Biol Psychiatry. 2011 Jun 15;69(12):e113-25. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.03.028. PMID: 21531388; PMCID: PMC3111448. REVIEW

  80. Arnsten, Pliszka (2011): Catecholamine influences on prefrontal cortical function: relevance to treatment of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and related disorders. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2011 Aug;99(2):211-6. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2011.01.020. PMID: 21295057; PMCID: PMC3129015. REVIEW

  81. Waldman, Rowe, Abramowitz, Kozel, Mohr, Sherman, Cleveland, Sanders, Gard, Stever (1998): Association and linkage of the dopamine transporter gene and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in children: heterogeneity owing to diagnostic subtype and severity. Am J Hum Genet. 1998 Dec;63(6):1767-76. doi: 10.1086/302132. PMID: 9837830; PMCID: PMC1377649.

  82. Wilens (2008): Effects of methylphenidate on the catecholaminergic system in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Clin Psychopharmacol 28 (3 Suppl 2), S. 46 – 53

  83. Volkow, Wang, Smith, Fowler, Telang, Logan, Tomasi (2015); Recovery of dopamine transporters with methamphetamine detoxification is not linked to changes in dopamine release. Neuroimage. 2015 Nov 1;121:20-8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.035.

  84. Krause, Dresel, Krause, la Fougere, Ackenheil (2003): The dopamine transporter and neuroimaging in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2003 Nov;27(7):605-13. n = 31

  85. Roessner, Sagvolden, Dasbanerjee, Middleton, Faraone, Walaas, Becker, Rothenberger, Bock (2010): Neuroscience. 2010 Jun 2;167(4):1183-91. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.02.073.

  86. Miller, Pomerleau, Huettl, Russell, Gerhardt, Glaser (2012): Neuropharmacology. The spontaneously hypertensive and Wistar Kyoto rat models of ADHD exhibit sub-regional differences in dopamine release and uptake in the striatum and nucleus accumbens. 2012 Dec;63(8):1327-34. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.08.020.

  87. Chen, Zheng, Xie, Huang, Ke, Zheng, Lu, Hu (2017): Glucocorticoids/glucocorticoid receptors effect on dopaminergic neurotransmitters in ADHD rats; Brain Research Bulletin; Volume 131, May 2017, Pages 214-220

  88. Beery, Quay, Pelham (2013): Differential Response to Methylphenidate in Inattentive and Combined Subtype ADHD; Journal of Attention Disorders Vol 21, Issue 1, pp. 62 – 70, https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054712469256

  89. Socanski, Herigstad, Beneventi, Einarsdottir (2015): ADHD predominantly inattentive subtype, interictal epileptiform discharges and use of methylphenidate for ADHD; European Journal of Paediatric Neurology, Volume 19, Supplement 1, May 2015, Page S75

  90. Waldman, Rowe, Abramowitz, Kozel, Mohr, Sherman, Cleveland, Sanders, Gard, Stever (1998): Association and linkage of the dopamine transporter gene and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in children: heterogeneity owing to diagnostic subtype and severity.Am J Hum Genet. 1998 Dec;63(6):1767-76.

  91. Park, Cho, Kim, Shin, Yoo, Oh, Han, Cheong, Kim (2014): Differential perinatal risk factors in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder by subtype. Psychiatry Res. 2014 Nov 30;219(3):609-16. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.05.036.

  92. Bidwell, Willcutt, McQueen, DeFries, Olson, Smith, Pennington (2011): A Family Based Association Study of DRD4, DAT1, and 5HTT and Continuous Traits of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder; Behav Genet. 2011 Jan; 41(1): 165–174. doi: 10.1007/s10519-010-9437-y, PMCID: PMC3674022, NIHMSID: NIHMS470165

  93. Akutagava-Martins, Salatino-Oliveira, Kieling, Genro, Polanczyk, Anselmi, Menezes, Gonçalves, Wehrmeister, Barros, Callegari-Jacques, Rohde, Hutz (2016): COMT and DAT1 genes are associated with hyperactivity and inattention traits in the 1993 Pelotas Birth Cohort: evidence of sex-specific combined effect. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2016 Oct;41(6):405-412. n = 4101

  94. Levy (2007): What do dopamine transporter and catechol-o-methyltransferase tell us about attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder? Pharmacogenomic implications. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2007 Jan;41(1):10-6.

  95. Al-Damluji (1988): Adrenergic mechanisms in the control of corticotrophin secretion. J Endocrinol 1988;119:5–14.

  96. Plotsky, Cunningham, Widmaier (1989): Catecholaminergic modulation of corticotropin-releasing factor and adrenocorticotropin secretion. Endocr Rev 1989;10:437–458.

  97. Oswald, Wong, McCaul, Zhou, Kuwabara, Choi, Brasic, Wand (2005): Relationships among ventral striatal dopamine release, cortisol secretion, and subjective responses to amphetamine. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005 Apr;30(4):821-32.

  98. Skosnik, Chatterton, Swisher, Park (2000): Modulation of attentional inhibition by norepinephrine and cortisol after psychological stress; International Journal of Psychophysiology 36 2000 59-68

  99. Gordis, Granger, Susman, Trickett (2006): Asymmetry between salivary cortisol and α-amylase reactivity to stress: Relation to aggressive behavior in adolescents, Psychoneuroendocrinology, Volume 31 , Issue 8 , 976 – 987

  100. Kao, Stalla, Stalla, Wotjak, Anderzhanova (2015): Norepinephrine and corticosterone in the medial prefrontal cortex and hippocampus predict PTSD-like symptoms in mice. Eur J Neurosci, 41: 1139–1148. doi:10.1111/ejn.12860

  101. Castellanos, Elia, Kruesi, Gulotta, Mefford, Potte, Ritchie, Rapoport (1994): Cerebrospinal fluid monoamine metabolites in boys with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder; Psychiatry Research, Volume 52, Issue 3, 305 – 316

  102. Higley, Suomi, Linnoila (1992): A longitudinal assessment of CSF monoamine metabolite and plasma cortisol concentrations in young rhesus monkeys, Biological Psychiatry, 1992 , Volume 32 , Issue 2 , 127 – 145, n = 22

  103. Wong, Kling, Munson, Listwak, Licinio, Prolo, Karp, McCutcheon, Geracioti, DeBellis, Rice, Goldstein, Veldhuis, Chrousos, Oldfield, McCann, Gold (2000): Pronounced and sustained central hypernoradrenergic function in major depression with melancholic features: Relation to hypercortisolism and corticotropin-releasing hormone. PNAS 2000 January, 97 (1) 325-330. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.97.1.325., n = 30

  104. Roy, Pickar, De Jong, Karoum, Linnoila (1988): Norepinephrine and Its Metabolites in Cerebrospinal Fluid, Plasma, and Urine – Relationship to Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis Function in Depression; Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988;45(9):849-857. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1988.01800330081010, n = 140

  105. Radosevich, Nash, Lacy, Brooks, O’Donovan, Williams, Abumrad (1989): Effects of low- and high-intensity exercise on plasma and cerebrospinal fluid levels of ir-β-endorphin, ACTH, cortisol, norepinephrine and glucose in the conscious dog; Brain Research, Volume 498, Issue 1, 25 September 1989, Pages 89-98

  106. Geracioti, Baker, Ekhator, West, Hill, Bruce, Schmidt, Rounds-Kugler, Yehuda, Keck, Kasckow (2001): CSF Norepinephrine Concentrations in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder; https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.158.8.1227

  107. de Kloet, Vermetten, Geuze, Kavelaars, Heijnen, Westenberg (2006): Assessment of HPA-axis function in posttraumatic stress disorder: pharmacological and non-pharmacological challenge tests, a review. J Psychiatr Res. 2006 Sep;40(6):550-67. Metaanalyse

  108. Geracioti, Baker1, Kasckow, Strawn, Mulchahey, Dashevsky, Horn, Ekhator (2008): Effects of trauma-related audiovisual stimulation on cerebrospinal fluid norepinephrine and corticotropin-releasing hormone concentrations in post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 2008 , Volume 33 , Issue 4 , 416 – 424, n = 8

  109. Meewisse, Reitsma, de Vries, Gersons, Olff (2007): Cortisol and post-traumatic stress disorder in adults. Systematic review and meta-analysis. The British Journal of Psychiatry Oct 2007, 191 (5) 387-392; DOI: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.024877, Metaanalyse von 37 Studien mit n = 1628

  110. Higley JD, Mehlman P, Taub DM, Higley SB, Suomi SJ, Linnoila M, Vickers JH. Cerebrospinal Fluid Monoamine and Adrenal Correlates of Aggression in Free-Ranging Rhesus Monkeys. *Arch Gen Psychiatry.*1992;49(6):436–441. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820060016002, n = 28

  111. Martignoni, Petraglia, Costa, Bono, Genazzani, Nappi (1990): Dementia of the Alzheimer type and hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenocortical axis: changes in cerebrospinal fluid corticotropin releasing factor and plasma cortisol levels. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 81: 452–456. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0404.1990.tb00994.x, n = 21

  112. Wang, Raskind, Wilkinson, Shofer, Sikkema, Szot, Quinn, Galasko, Peskind (2018): Associations between CSF cortisol and CSF norepinephrine in cognitively normal controls and patients with amnestic MCI and AD dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2018;1–6. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.4856

  113. Overli, Harris, Winberg (1999): Short-Term Effects of Fights for Social Dominance and the Establishment of Dominant-Subordinate Relationships on Brain Monoamines and Cortisol in Rainbow Trout. Brain Behav Evol 1999;54:263-275

  114. Radosevich, Lacy, Brown, Williams, Abumrad (1988): Effects of insulin-induced hypoglycemia on plasma and cerebrospinal fluid levels of ir-β-endorphins, ACTH, cortisol, norepinephrine, insulin and glucose in the conscious dog. Brain Research, Volume 458, Issue 2, 23 August 1988, Pages 325-338

  115. Breslow, Parker, Frank, Norris, Yates, Raff, Rock, Christopherson, Rosenfeld, Beattie (1993): Determinants of catecholamine and cortisol responses to lower extremity revascularization. The PIRAT Study Group. (PMID:8267195); Anesthesiology [01 Dec 1993, 79(6):1202-1209]

  116. http://www.lab4more.de/wp-content/uploads/2017/07/8006_Neurostress_I_NT_%20Metoboliten.pdf

  117. Engert, Vogel, Efanov, Duchesne, Corbo, Ali, Pruessner (2010): Investigation into the cross-correlation of salivary cortisol and alpha-amylase responses to psychological stress; Psychoneuroendocrinology , Volume 36 , Issue 9 , 1294 – 1302; DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.02.018, n = 50

  118. Strittmatter, Hamann, Blaes, Fischer, Grauer, Hoffmann, Schimrigk (2007): Fehlregulation der hypothalamisch-hypophysär-adrenalen Achse und chronobiologische Auffälligkeiten beim Clusterkopfschmerz; Alterations of the Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal Axis and Chronobiological Disturbances in Cluster Headache; Fortschr Neurol Psychiatr 1997; 65(1): 01-07; DOI: 10.1055/s-2007-996303

  119. Goenjian; Yehuda et al (1996): Basal cortisol, dexamethasone suppression of cortisol, and MHPG in adolescents after the 1988 earthquake in Armenia; The American Journal of Psychiatry; Washington Vol. 153, Iss. 7, (Jul 1996): 929-34

  120. Jimerson, Insel, Reus, Kopin (1983): Increased Plasma MHPG in Dexamethasone-Resistant Depressed Patients; Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1983;40(2):173-176. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1983.01790020067006

  121. Rubin, Price, Charney, Heninger (1985): Noradrenergic function and the cortisol response to dexamethasone in depression; Psychiatry Research, Volume 15, Issue 1, 5 – 15

  122. Yehuda, Resnick, Schmeidler, Yang, Pitman (1998): Predictors of Cortisol and 3-Methoxy-4-Hydroxyphenylglycol Responses in the Acute Aftermath of Rape; Biological Psychiatry, Volume 43, Issue 11, 855 – 859

  123. Uhde, Joffe, Jimerson, Post (1988): Normal urinary free cortisol and plasma MHPG in panic disorder: Clinical and theoretical implications; Biological Psychiatry Volume 23, Issue 6, 15 March 1988, Pages 575-585; n = 24

  124. Posener, Schildkraut, Samson, Schatzberg (1996): Diurnal variation of plasma cortisol and homovanillic acid in healthy subjects; Psychoneuroendocrinology, Volume 21, Issue 1, 33 – 38, n = 10

  125. Kopin, Jimerson, Markey, Ebert, Polinsky (1984): Disposition and Metabolism of MHPG in Humans: Application to Studies in Depression; Verteilung und Metabolismus des MHPG im menschlichen Organismus: Anwendung bei Studien zur Depression; Pharmacopsychiatry 1984; 17(1): 3-8; DOI: 10.1055/s-2007-1017399

  126. Blombery, Kopin, Gordon, Markey, Ebert (1980): Conversion of MHPG to Vanillylmandelic Acid – Implications for the Importance of Urinary MHPG. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1980;37(10):1095-1098. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1980.01780230013001

  127. Brennan, Arnsten (2008): Neuronal mechanisms underlying attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: the influence of arousal on prefrontal cortical function. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1129:236-45. doi: 10.1196/annals.1417.007.

  128. Pasion, Barbosa (2019): ERN as a transdiagnostic marker of the internalizing-externalizing spectrum: A dissociable meta-analytic effect. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019 Aug;103:133-149. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.06.013.

  129. Loo, McGough, McCracken, Smalley (2018): Parsing heterogeneity in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder using EEG-based subgroups. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2018 Mar;59(3):223-231. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12814, n = 781

  130. Bazanova, Auer, Sapina (2018): On the Efficiency of In dividualized Theta/Beta Ratio Neurofeedback Combined with Forehead EMG Training in ADHD Children. Front Hum Neurosci. 2018 Jan 18;12:3. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00003. eCollection 2018., n = 117

  131. Diamond: Attention-deficit disorder (attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder without hyperactivity): A neurobiologically and behaviorally distinct disorder from attention-deficit (with hyperactivity), Development and Psychopathology 17 (2005), 807–825, S. 819, Seite 810

  132. Bussalb, Collin, Barthélemy, Ojeda, Bioulac, Blasco-Fontecilla, Brandeis, Purper Ouakil, Ros, Mayaud (2019): Is there a cluster of high theta-beta ratio patients in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder? Clin Neurophysiol. 2019 Aug;130(8):1387-1396. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.02.021.

  133. Clarke, Barry, Dupuy, Heckel, McCarthy, Selikowitz, Johnstone (2011): Behavioural differences between EEG-defined subgroups of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology, 122, 1333-1341., dort: “Cluster 4”, n = 264

  134. Clarke, Barry, McCarthy, Selikowitz (1998): EEG analysis in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a comparative study of two subtypes. Psychiatry Res 1998;81:19–29.

  135. Clarke, Barry, McCarthy, Selikowitz (2001): EEG differences in two subtypes of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Psychophysiology, 2001; 38:212–21

  136. Kühle, Dr. med Hans-Jürgen, Neurofeedbacktherapie bei ADHS, 2010 (PDF, Download August 2015), Kapitel 8 oder Kühle (2010) Neurofeedbacktherapie bei ADHS, hier Kapitel 11

  137. Kühle (2010): Neurofeedbacktherapie bei ADHS (PDF, Download August 2015), Kapitel 8 oder Kühle (2010): Neurofeedbacktherapie bei ADHS, hier Kapitel 11

  138. Clarke, Barry, Dupuy, Heckel, McCarthy, Selikowitz, Johnstone (2011): Behavioural differences between EEG-defined subgroups of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology, 122, 1333-1341., n = 264

  139. Gevensleben, Moll, Heinrich (2010): Neurofeedback-Training bei Kindern mit Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit-/Hyperaktivitätsstörung (ADHS); Effekte auf Verhaltens- und neurophysiologischer Ebene; Zeitschrift für Kinder- und Jugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 38 (6), 2010, 409–420, Seite 417

  140. Mohagheghi, Amiri, Moghaddasi Bonab, Chalabianloo, Noorazar, Tabatabaei, Farhang (2017): A Randomized Trial of Comparing the Efficacy of Two Neurofeedback Protocols for Treatment of Clinical and Cognitive Symptoms of ADHD: Theta Suppression/Beta Enhancement and Theta Suppression/Alpha Enhancement. Biomed Res Int. 2017;2017:3513281. doi: 10.1155/2017/3513281.

  141. Lubar, J. F., Swartwood, M. O., Swartwood, J. N. & O’Donnell, P. H. (1995). Evaluation of the effectiveness of EEG neurofeedback training for ADHD in a clinical setting as measured by changes in T.O.V.A. scores, behavioral ratings, and WISC-R performance. Biofeedback and Self Regulation 20, 83–99 n = 19

  142. Clarke, Barry, Dupuy, Heckel, McCarthy, Selikowitz, Johnstone (2011): Behavioural differences between EEG-defined subgroups of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology, 122, 1333-1341., n = 264, dort als “Cluster 1”

  143. Kühle (2010): Neurofeedbacktherapie bei ADHS (PDF, Download August 2015), Kapitel 8 oder Kühle (2010) Neurofeedbacktherapie bei ADHS, hier Kapitel 11

  144. Kropotov (2008): Quantitative EEG, Event-Related Potentials and Neurotherapy, zitiert nach Strehl (Herausgeber) (2013): Neurofeedback, Seite 60. Kropotov ist einer der angesehensten Forscher auf dem Gebiet von QEEG und AD(H)S. Er ist Leiter des Institute of human brain in St. Petersburg, an dem bereits Pawlow tätig war.

  145. Kropotov (2008): Quantitative EEG, Event-Related Potentials and Neurotherapy., zitiert nach Strehl (Herausgeber) (2013): Neurofeedback, Seite 60

  146. Clarke, Barry, McCarthy, Selikowitz (2001): Excess beta in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: an atypical electrophysiological group. Psychiatry Research, 103, 205-218.

  147. Clarke, Barry, Dupuy, McCarthy, Selikowitz, Johnstone (2013). Excess beta activity in the EEG of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a disorder of arousal? International Journal of Psychophysiology, 89, 314-319

  148. Ogrim, Kropotov, Brunner, Candrian, Sandvik, Hestad (2014): Predicting the clinical outcome of stimulant medication in pediatric attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: data from quantitative electroencephalography, event-related potentials, and a go/no-go test. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2014 Feb 3;10:231-42. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S56600. N = 188

  149. Clarke, Barry, Dupuy, Heckel, McCarthy, Selikowitz, Johnstone (2011): Behavioural differences between EEG-defined subgroups of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology, 122, 1333-1341., n = 264, dort: “Cluster 3”

  150. Kropotov (2008): Quantitative EEG, Event-Related Potentials and Neurotherapy,, zitiert nach Strehl (Herausgeber) (2013): Neurofeedback, Seite 60. Kropotov ist einer der angesehensten Forscher auf dem Gebiet von QEEG und AD(H)S. Er ist Leiter des Institute of human brain in St. Petersburg, an dem bereits Pawlow tätig war.

  151. Kropotov (2008): Quantitative EEG, Event-Related Potentials and Neurotherapy., zitiert nach Strehl (Herausgeber) (2013): Neurofeedback, Seite 60

  152. Kropotov (2008): Quantitative EEG, Event-Related Potentials and Neurotherapy,, zitiert nach Strehl (Herausgeber) (2013): Neurofeedback, Seite 60

  153. Kropotov (2008): Quantitative EEG, Event-Related Potentials and Neurotherapy, zitiert nach Strehl (Herausgeber) (2013): Neurofeedback, Seite 60

  154. Clarke, Barry, Dupuy, Heckel, McCarthy, Selikowitz, Johnstone (2011): Behavioural differences between EEG-defined subgroups of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology, 122, 1333-1341., Seite 1339

  155. Sutton, Burnette, Mundy, Meyer, Vaughan, Sanders, Yale (2005): Resting cortical brain activity and social behavior in higher functioning children with autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2005;46:211–22.

  156. Chang, Yang, Chiang, Ouyang, Wu, Yu, Lin. Delay Maturation in Occipital Lobe in Girls With Inattention Subtype of Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Clin EEG Neurosci. 2020 Jan 14;1550059419899328. doi: 10.1177/1550059419899328. PMID: 31933379.

  157. Kropotov (2008): Quantitative EEG, Event-Related Potentials and Neurotherapy., zitiert nach Strehl (Herausgeber) (2013): Neurofeedback, Seite 60. Kropotov ist einer der angesehensten Forscher auf dem Gebiet von QEEG und AD(H)S. Er ist Leiter des Institute of human brain in St. Petersburg, an dem bereits Pawlow tätig war.

  158. Müller, Candrian, Kropotov (2011): ADHS – Neurofeedback in der Praxis, S. 157

  159. hbimed, abgerufen Herbst 2015, nicht mehr online

  160. Ji Y, Choi TY, Lee J, Yoon S, Won GH, Jeong H, Kang SW, Kim JW (2022): Characteristics of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Subtypes in Children Classified Using Quantitative Electroencephalography. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2022 Nov 21;18:2725-2736. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S386774. PMID: 36437880; PMCID: PMC9697401.

  161. Duric NS, Assmus J, Børresen H, Golos AD, Socanski D, Duric A, Surmeli T (2023): Quantitative electroencephalography in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and healthy children: Behavioral and age correlates. Appl Neuropsychol Child. 2023 Dec 12:1-9. doi: 10.1080/21622965.2023.2288865. PMID: 38086349.

  162. Lei, Ma, Du, Shen, Jin, Gong (2014): Microstructural abnormalities in the combined and inattentive subtypes of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a diffusion tensor imaging study. In: Scientific reports. Band 4, 2014, S. 6875. n = 21 ADHS-C und n = 28 ADHS-HI

  163. Witt, Stevens (2015): Relationship between white matter microstructure abnormalities and ADHD symptomatology in adolescents. Psychiatry research. Band 232, Nummer 2, Mai 2015, S. 168–174. n = 22

  164. Oades (2008): Dopamine–serotonin interactions in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), Progress in Brain Research, Volume 172, 2008, Pages 543-565, https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(08)00926-6

  165. Serrallach, Groß, Christiner, Wildermuth, Schneider (2022): Neuromorphological and Neurofunctional Correlates of ADHD and ADD in the Auditory Cortex of Adults. Front Neurosci. 2022 May 6;16:850529. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.850529. PMID: 35600622; PMCID: PMC9121124. n = 82

  166. Hahn, Heinzel, Dresler, Plichta, Renner, Markulin, Jakob, Lesch, Fallgatter (2011): Association between reward-related activation in the ventral striatum and trait reward sensitivity is moderated by dopamine transporter genotype. Hum Brain Mapp. Oct;32(10):1557-65. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21127., zitiert nach Chiossi (2013): Neuronale Grundlagen der Persönlichkeit nach Gray: Ein Vergleich von Ego-Shooter-Spielern und -Nicht-Spielern, Dissertation, Seite 23

  167. Hahn, Dresler, Plichta, Ehlis, Ernst, Markulin, Polak, Blaimer, Deckert, Lesch, Jakob, Fallgatter (2010): Functional Amygdala-Hippocampus Connectivity During Anticipation of Aversive Events is Associated with Gray’s Trait “Sensitivity to Punishment”; DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.04.033, zitiert nach Chiossi (2013): Neuronale Grundlagen der Persönlichkeit nach Gray: Ein Vergleich von Ego-Shooter-Spielern und -Nicht-Spielern, Dissertation, Seite 24